"It's Always the Judge's Fault": Attention, Emotion Recognition, and Expertise in Rhythmic Gymnastics Assessment
Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články
PubMed
27458406
PubMed Central
PMC4932815
DOI
10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01008
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- Klíčová slova
- accuracy, attention, emotion recognition, expertise, judges, rhythmic gymnastics,
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
In many sports, such as figure skating or gymnastics, the outcome of a performance does not rely exclusively on objective measurements, but on more subjective cues. Judges need high attentional capacities to process visual information and overcome fatigue. Also their emotion recognition abilities might have an effect in detecting errors and making a more accurate assessment. Moreover, the scoring given by judges could be also influenced by their level of expertise. This study aims to assess how rhythmic gymnastics judges' emotion recognition and attentional abilities influence accuracy of performance assessment. Data will be collected from rhythmic gymnastics judges and coaches at different international levels. This study will employ an online questionnaire consisting on an emotion recognition test and attentional test. Participants' task is to watch a set of videotaped rhythmic gymnastics performances and evaluate them on the artistic and execution components of performance. Their scoring will be compared with the official scores given at the competition the video was taken from to measure the accuracy of the participants' evaluations. The proposed research represents an interdisciplinary approach that integrates cognitive and sport psychology within experimental and applied contexts. The current study advances the theoretical understanding of how emotional and attentional aspects affect the evaluation of sport performance. The results will provide valuable evidence on the direction and strength of the relationship between the above-mentioned factors and the accuracy of sport performance evaluation. Importantly, practical implications might be drawn from this study. Intervention programs directed at improving the accuracy of judges could be created based on the understanding of how emotion recognition and attentional abilities are related to the accuracy of performance assessment.
Eötvös Loránd University Budapest Hungary
Maastricht University Maastricht Netherlands
Masaryk University Brno Czech Republic
University of Banja Luka Banja Luka Bosnia and Herzegovina
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Adan A. (1991). Influence of morningness–eveningness preference in the relationship between body temperature and performance: a diurnal study. Pers. Individ. Dif. 12 1159–1169. 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90080-U DOI
Adan A., Almirall H. (1991). Horne & Östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: a reduced scale. Pers. Individ. Dif. 12 241–253. 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W DOI
Adan A., Archer S. N., Hidalgo M. P., Di Milia L., Natale V., Randler C. (2012). Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol. Int. 29 1153–1175. 10.3109/07420528.2012.719971 PubMed DOI
Adan A., Guàrdia J. (1993). Circadian variations of self-reported activation: a multidimensional approach. Chronobiologia 20 233–244. PubMed
Ansorge C. J., Scheer J. K. (1988). International bias detected in judging gymnastic competition at the 1984 Olympic Games. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 59 103–107. 10.1080/02701367.1988.10605486 DOI
Antonakis J., Bastardoz N. (2013). Swain: Stata Module to Correct the SEM Chi-Square Overidentification Test in Small Sample Sizes or Complex Models. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College Department of Economics.
Antonakis J., Bendahan S., Jacquart P., Lalive R. (2010). On making causal claims: a review and recommendations. Leadersh. Q. 21 1086–1120. 10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.010 DOI
Bai L., Lao S., Zhang W., Jones G. J., Smeaton A. F. (2007). “A semantic event detection approach for soccer video based on perception concepts and finiste state machines,” in Proceedings of the Eight International Workshop on Image Analysis for Multimedia Interactive Services (Washington, DC: IEEE Computer Society; ), 30 10.1109/wiamis.2007.12 DOI
Baltagi B. H. (2011). Econometrics. New York, NY: Springer; 10.1007/978-3-642-20059-5 DOI
Blake R., Shiffrar M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58 47–73. 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152 PubMed DOI
Browne M. W., Cudeck R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models, eds Bollen K. A., Long J. S. (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications; ), 136–162.
Caci H., Nadalet L., Staccini P., Myquel M., Boyer P. (2000). The composite scale of morningness: further psychometric properties and temporal stability. Eur. Psychiatry 15 278–281. 10.1016/S0924-9338(00)00231-5 PubMed DOI
Catteeuw P., Helsen W., Gilis B., Wagemans J. (2009). Decision-making skills, role specificity, and deliberate practice in association football refereeing. J. Sports Sci. 27 1125–1136. 10.1080/02640410903079179 PubMed DOI
Cavanagh P., Alvarez G. A. (2005). Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9 349–354. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.009 PubMed DOI
Clarisse R., Le Floch N., Kindelberger C., Feunteun P. (2010). Daily rhythmicity of attention in morning- vs. evening-type adolescents at boarding school unter different psychosociological testing conditions. Chronobiol. Int. 27 826–841. 10.3109/07420521003794051 PubMed DOI
Correa Á., Molina E., Sanabria D. (2014). Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 67 113–118. 10.1016/j.aap.2014.02.020 PubMed DOI
Dosseville F., Laborde S., Garncerzyk C. (2014). “Current research in sports officiating and decision-making,” in Contemporary Topics and Trends in the Psychology of Sports, ed. Mohiyeddini C. (New York, NY: Nova Publishers; ), 13–38.
Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., Buchner A. (2007). G∗Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39 175–191. 10.3758/BF03193146 PubMed DOI
Fédération Internationale de Gymnastique [FIG] (2013). Code of Points Rhythmic Gymnastics 2013–2016. Lausanne: Fédération Internationale de Gymnastique.
Fernandez-Villarino M. A., Bobo-Arce M., Sierra-Palmeiro E. (2013). Practical skills of rhythmic gymnastics judges. J. Hum. Kinet. 39 243–249. 10.2478/hukin-2013-0087 PubMed DOI PMC
Flessas K., Mylonas D., Panagiotaropoulou G., Tsopani D., Korda A., Siettos C., et al. (2015). Judging the judges’ performance in rhythmic gymnastics. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 47 640–648. 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000425 PubMed DOI
Gabrielsson A., Juslin P. N. (1996). Emotional expression in music performance: between the performer’s intention and the listener’s experience. Psychol. Music 24 68–91. 10.1177/0305735696241007 DOI
Glisky E. L. (2007). “Changes in cognitive function in human aging,” in Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms: Frontiers in Neuroscience, ed. Riddle D. R. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ), 3–20.
Gomes A. A., Tavares J., deAzevedo M. H. P. (2011). Sleep and academic performance in undergraduates: a multi-measure, multi-predictor approach. Chronobiol. Int. 28 786–801. 10.3109/07420528.2011.606518 PubMed DOI
Greenwood K. M. (1994). Long-term stability and psychometric properties of the Composite Scale of Morningness. Ergonomics 37 377–383. 10.1080/00140139408963653 PubMed DOI
Hagemann N., Strauss B., Leißing J. (2008). When the referee sees red. Psychol. Sci. 19 769–771. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02155.x PubMed DOI
James L. R., Singh B. K. (1978). An introduction to the logic, assumptions, and basic analytic procedures of two-stage least squares. Psychol. Bull. 85 1104–1122. 10.1037/0033-2909.85.5.1104 DOI
Lazarus R. S. (2000). How emotions influence performance in competitive sport. Sport Psychol. 14 229–252.
Linacre J. M. (1999). Estimation methods for Rasch measures. J. Outcome Meas. 3 382–405. PubMed
Looney M. A. (2003). Evaluating judge performance in sport. J. Appl. Meas. 5 31–47. PubMed
Loula F., Prasad S., Harber K., Shiffrar M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31 210–220. 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.210 PubMed DOI
Nevill A. M., Balmer N. J., Williams A. M. (2002). The influence of crowd noise and experience upon refereeing decisions in football. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 3 261–272. 10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00033-4 DOI
Oksama L., Hyönä J. (2004). Is multiple object tracking carried out automatically by an early vision mechanism independent of higher-order cognition? An individual difference approach. Vis. Cogn. 11 631–671. 10.1080/13506280344000473 DOI
Oudejans R. R., Verheijen R., Bakker F. C., Gerrits J. C., Steinbruckner M., Beek P. J. (2000). Errors in judging ‘offside’ in football. Nature 404 33 10.1038/35003639 PubMed DOI
Plessner H. (1999). Expectation biases in gymnastics judging. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 21 131–144.
Plessner H., Schallies E. (2005). Judging the cross on rings: a matter of achieving shape constancy. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 19 1145–1156. 10.1002/acp.1136 DOI
Roca J. (2012). Measuring the Three Attentional Networks in Vigilance Context and their Relationship with Driving Behaviuor. Doctoral dissesrtation, Universitiy of Granada, Granada.
Roca J., Castro C., López-Ramón M. F., Lupiáñez J. (2011). Measuring vigilance while assessing the functioning of the three attentional networks: the ANTI-Vigilance task. J. Neurosci. Methods 298 312–324. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.04.014 PubMed DOI
Rodenberg R. (2011). Perception? Reality: analyzing specific allegations of NBA referee bias. J. Quant. Anal. Sports 7 8 10.2202/1559-0410.1326 DOI
Schlegel K., Grandjean D., Scherer K. R. (2014). Introducing the geneva emotion recognition test: an example of rasch-based test development. Psychol. Assess. 26 666–672. 10.1037/a0035246 PubMed DOI
Schlegel K., Scherer K. R. (2015). Introducing a short version of the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test (GERT-S): psychometric properties and construct validation. Behav. Res. Methods 47 1–10. PubMed
Schmidt C., Peigneux P., Leclercq Y., Sterpenich V., Vandewalle G., Phillips C., et al. (2012). Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS ONE 7:e29658 10.1371/journal.pone.0029658 PubMed DOI PMC
Smith C. S., Reilly C., Midkiff K. (1989). Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J. Appl. Psychol. 74 728–738. 10.1037/0021-9010.74.5.728 PubMed DOI
Ste-Marie D. M. (1999). Expert-novice differences in gymnastic judging: an information-processing perspective. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 13 269–281. 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199906)13:3<269::AID-ACP567>3.0.CO;2-Y DOI
Ste-Marie D. M. (2000). Expertise in women’s gymnastic judging: an observational approach. Percept. Mot. Skills 90 543–546. 10.2466/pms.2000.90.2.543 PubMed DOI
Steiger J. H., Lind J. (1980). Statistically-based tests for the number of common factors. Paper Presented at the Annual Spring Meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.
Vast R. L., Young R. L., Thomas P. R. (2010). Emotions in sport: perceived effects on attention, concentration, and performance. Aust. Psychol. 45 132–140. 10.1080/00050060903261538 DOI
Wright B. D., Mok M. (2000). Rasch models overview. J. Appl. Meas. 1 83–106. PubMed