Predictors of Bribe-Taking: The Role of Bribe Size and Personality

. 2018 ; 9 () : 1511. [epub] 20180910

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid30250440

Laboratory studies allow studying the predictors of bribe-taking in a controlled setting. However, presently used laboratory tasks often lack any connection to norm violation or invite participants to role-play. A new experimental task for studying the decision to take a bribe was designed in this study to overcome these problems by embedding the opportunity for bribe-taking in an unrelated task that participants perform. Using this new experimental task, we found that refraining from harming a third party by taking a bribe was associated with lower offered bribes and higher scores of the participants on the honesty-humility scale from the HEXACO personality inventory. A trial-level analysis showed that response times were longer for trials with bribes and even longer for trials in which bribes were accepted. These results suggest that taking a bribe may require overcoming automatic honest response and support the validity of the honesty-humility scale in predicting moral behavior.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Abbink K. (2004). Staff rotation as an anti-corruption policy: an experimental study. Eur. J. Polit. Econ. 20 887–906. 10.1016/j.ejpoleco.2003.10.008 DOI

Abbink K. (2006). “Laboratory experiments on corruption,” in International Handbook on the Economics of Corruption, ed. Rose-Ackerman S. (Cheltenham: Elgar; ), 418–437.

Abbink K., Irlenbusch B., Renner E. (2002). An experimental bribery game. J. Law Econ. Org. 18 428–454. 10.1093/jleo/18.2.428 PubMed DOI

Abbink K., Serra D. (2012). “Anticorruption policies: lessons from the lab,” in New Advances in Experimental Research on Corruption. Research in Experimental Economics, eds Serra D., Wantchekon L. (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing; ), 77–115. 10.1108/S0193-2306(2012)0000015006 DOI

Alatas V., Cameron L., Chaudhuri A., Erkal N., Gangadharan L. (2009a). Gender, culture, and corruption: insights from an experimental analysis. South. Econ. J. 75 663–680.

Alatas V., Cameron L., Chaudhuri A., Erkal N., Gangadharan L. (2009b). Subject pool effects in a corruption experiment: a comparison of Indonesian public servants and Indonesian students. Exp. Econ. 12 113–132. 10.1007/s10683-008-9207-3 DOI

Armantier O., Bóly A. (2012). “On the external validity of laboratory experiments on corruption,” in New Advances in Experimental Research on Corruption. Research in Experimental Economics, eds Serra D., Wantchekon L. (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing; ), 117–144.

Baayen R. H., Davidson D. J., Bates D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J. Mem. Lang. 59 390–412. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 DOI

Baayen R. H., Milin P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 3 12–28. 10.21500/20112084.807 DOI

Banuri S., Eckel C. (2012). “Experiments in culture and corruption: a review,” in New Advances in Experimental Research on Corruption. Research in Experimental Economics, eds Serra D., Wantchekon L. (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing; ), 51–76. 10.1108/S0193-2306(2012)0000015005 DOI

Barr A., Serra D. (2009). The effects of externalities and framing on bribery in a petty corruption experiment. Exp. Econ. 12 488–503. 10.1007/s10683-009-9225-9 DOI

Barr A., Serra D. (2010). Corruption and culture: an experimental analysis. J. Public Econ. 94 862–869. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2010.07.006 DOI

Becker A., Deckers T., Dohmen T., Falk A., Kosse F. (2012). The relationship between economic preferences and psychological personality measures. Ann. Rev. Econ. 4 453–478. 10.1146/annurev-economics-080511-110922 DOI

Blanken I., van de Ven N., Zeelenberg M. (2015). A meta-analytic review of moral licensing. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41 540–558. 10.1177/0146167215572134 PubMed DOI

Bobkova N., Egbert H. (2013). Corruption investigated in the lab: a survey of the experimental literature. Int. J. Latest Trends Finance Econ. Sci. 2 337–349.

Bouwmeester S., Verkoeijen P. P., Aczel B., Barbosa F., Bègue L., Brañas-Garza P., et al. (2017). Registered replication report: rand, Greene, and Nowak (2012). Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12 527–542. 10.1177/1745691617693624 PubMed DOI PMC

Camerer C., Thaler R. H. (1995). Anomalies: ultimatums, dictators and manners. J. Econ. Perspect. 9 209–219. 10.1257/jep.9.2.209 DOI

Cameron L., Chaudhuri A., Erkal N., Gangadharan L. (2009). Propensities to engage in and punish corrupt behavior: experimental evidence from Australia, India, Indonesia and Singapore. J. Public Econ. 93 843–851. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2009.03.004 DOI

Chaudhuri A. (2012). “Gender and corruption: a survey of the experimental evidence,” in New Advances in Experimental Research on Corruption. Research in Experimental Economics, eds Serra D., Wantchekon L. (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing; ), 13–49. 10.1108/S0193-2306(2012)0000015004 DOI

D’Adda G., Drouvelis M., Nosenzo D. (2016). Norm elicitation in within-subject designs: testing for order effects. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 62 1–7. 10.1016/j.socec.2016.02.003 DOI

Dai Z., Galeotti F., Villeval M. C. (2017). Cheating in the lab predicts fraud in the field: an experiment in public transportation. Manage. Sci. 64 1081–1100. 10.1287/mnsc.2016.2616 DOI

Dollar D., Fisman R., Gatti R. (2001). Are women really the “fairer” sex? Corruption and women in government. J. Econ. Behav. Org. 46 423–429. 10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00169-X DOI

Drugov M., Hamman J., Serra D. (2014). Intermediaries in corruption: an experiment. Exp. Econ. 17 78–99. 10.1007/s10683-013-9358-8 DOI

Effron D. A., Conway P. (2015). When virtue leads to villainy: advances in research on moral self-licensing. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 6 32–35. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.017 DOI

Fišar M., Kubák M., Špalek J., Tremewan J. (2016). Gender differences in beliefs and actions in a framed corruption experiment. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 63 69–82. 10.1016/j.socec.2016.05.004 DOI

Fischbacher U., Föllmi-Heusi F. (2013). Lies in disguise—An experimental study on cheating. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 11 525–547. 10.1111/jeea.12014 DOI

Gelman A., Hill J. (2006). Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 10.1017/CBO9780511790942 DOI

Hilbig B. E., Thielmann I., Wührl J., Zettler I. (2015). From honesty–humility to fair behavior–benevolence or a (blind) fairness norm? Pers. Individ. Differ. 80 91–95. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.017 DOI

Hilbig B. E., Zettler I. (2009). Pillars of cooperation: honesty–humility, social value orientations, and economic behavior. J. Res. Pers. 43 516–519. 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.003 DOI

Hilbig B. E., Zettler I. (2015). When the cat’s away, some mice will play: a basic trait account of dishonest behavior. J. Res. Pers. 57 72–88. 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.04.003 PubMed DOI

Houdek P. (2017). Is behavioral ethics ready for giving business and policy advice? J. Manag. Inquiry. 10.1177/1056492617712894 DOI

Jain A. K. (2001). Corruption: a review. J. Econ. Surv. 15 71–121. 10.1111/1467-6419.00133 DOI

Köbis N. C., van Prooijen J. W., Righetti F., Van Lange P. A. (2015). “Who doesn’t?”—The impact of descriptive norms on corruption. PLoS One 10:e0131830. 10.1371/journal.pone.0131830 PubMed DOI PMC

Lee K., Ashton M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behav. Res. 39 329–358. 10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8 PubMed DOI

Lee K., Ashton M. C., de Vries R. E. (2005). Predicting workplace delinquency and integrity with the HEXACO and five-factor models of personality structure. Hum. Perf. 18 179–197. 10.1207/s15327043hup1802_4 DOI

Levitt S. D., List J. A. (2007). What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? J. Econ. Perspect. 21 153–174. 10.1257/jep.21.2.153 DOI

Mazar N., Ariely D. (2006). Dishonesty in everyday life and its policy implications. J. Public Policy Mark. 25 117–126. 10.1509/jppm.25.1.117 DOI

McCrae R. R., Costa P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am. Psychol. 52 509–516. 10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509 PubMed DOI

Potters J., Stoop J. (2016). Do cheaters in the lab also cheat in the field? Eur. Econ. Rev. 87 26–33. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2016.03.004 DOI

Rand D. G., Greene J. D., Nowak M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature 489 427–430. 10.1038/nature11467 PubMed DOI

Rosenbaum S. M., Billinger S., Stieglitz N. (2014). Let’s be honest: a review of experimental evidence of honesty and truth-telling. J. Econ. Psychol. 45 181–196. 10.1016/j.joep.2014.10.002 DOI

Shaw P., Vásquez W. F., LeClair M. (2013). Intelligence and bribing behavior in a one-shot game. J. Socio Econ. 44 91–96. 10.1016/j.socec.2013.02.013 DOI

van Veldhuizen R. (2013). The influence of wages on public officials’ corruptibility: a laboratory investigation. J. Econ. Psychol. 39 341–356. 10.1016/j.joep.2013.09.009 DOI

Ward D. A., Beck W. L. (1990). Gender and dishonesty. J. Soc. Psychol. 130 333–339. 10.1080/00224545.1990.9924589 DOI

Weaver G. R., Reynolds S. J., Brown M. E. (2014). Moral intuition: connecting current knowledge to future organizational research and practice. J. Manag. 40 100–129. 10.1177/0149206313511272 DOI

Weisel O., Shalvi S. (2015). The collaborative roots of corruption. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 10651–10656. 10.1073/pnas.1423035112 PubMed DOI PMC

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

No evidence of moral licensing in a laboratory bribe-taking task

. 2022 Aug 16 ; 12 (1) : 13860. [epub] 20220816

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...