Colour Categorization and its Effect on Perception: A Conceptual Replication

. 2023 Feb ; 52 (1) : 1-16. [epub] 20210724

Jazyk angličtina Země Spojené státy americké Médium print-electronic

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid34304342
Odkazy

PubMed 34304342
DOI 10.1007/s10936-021-09791-2
PII: 10.1007/s10936-021-09791-2
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje

The presented study examines the question of colour categorization in relation to the hypothesis of linguistic relativity. The study is based on research conducted by Gilbert et al. (2006) and their claim that linguistic colour categorization in a particular language helps colour recognition and speeds the process of colour discrimination for colours from different linguistic categories but only for the right visual field. Our study approached the research question differently. We used the same methodology as Gilbert's team et al. (2006), but we used different colour categories in the Czech language and significantly enlarged the number of participants to 106 undergraduate psychology students. Our results show that the fastest reaction times were in trials when the target was located in the left visual field, quite opposite from the Gilbert's et al. (2006) study. We argue that this finding is based on different processes than simple colour linguistic categorisation and attentional processes actually play an important role in the task.

Erratum v

PubMed

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Appelbaum, M., Cooper, H., Kline, R. B., Mayo-Wilson, E., Nezu, A. M., & Rao, S. M. (2018). Journal article reporting standards for quantitative research in psychology: The APA Publications and Communications Board task force report. American Psychologist, 73(1), 3–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000191 PubMed DOI

Bartolomeo, P. (2006). A parietofrontal network for spatial awareness in the right hemisphere of the human brain. Archives of Neurology, 63(9), 1238–1241. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.63.9.1238 PubMed DOI

Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (2002). Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(2), 217–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00065-3 DOI

Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1991). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Univ. of California Press.

Casasanto, D. (2008). Who’s afraid of the big bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic differences in temporal language and thought. Language Learning, 58, 63–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2008.00462.x DOI

Chica, A. B., de Schotten, M. T., Toba, M., Malhotra, P., Lupiáñez, J., & Bartolomeo, P. (2012). Attention networks and their interactions after right-hemisphere damage. Cortex, 48(6), 654–663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.01.009 PubMed DOI

Daoutis, C. A., Franklin, A., Riddett, A., Clifford, A., & Davies, I. R. (2006a). Categorical effects in children’s colour search: A cross-linguistic comparison. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 373–400. DOI

Daoutis, C. A., Pilling, M., & Davies, I. R. (2006b). Categorical effects in visual search for colour. Visual Cognition, 14(2), 217–240. DOI

Davies, I. R., & Corbett, G. G. (1994). The basic colour terms of Russian. Linguistics, 32(1), 65–90. DOI

Delta–E Calculator. (n.d.). ColorMine. http://colormine.org/delta-e-calculator .

Drivonikou, G. V., Kay, P., Regier, T., Ivry, R. B., Gilbert, A. L., Franklin, A., & Davies, I. R. L. (2007). Further evidence that Whorfian effects are stronger in the right visual field than the left. PNAS, 104(3), 1097–1102. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610132104 PubMed DOI PMC

Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Clifford, A., Kay, P., Regier, T., & Davies, I. R. L. (2008). Lateralization of categorical perception of colour changes with colour term acquisition. PNAS, 105(47), 18221–18225. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809952105 PubMed DOI PMC

Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. PNAS, 103(2), 489–494. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0509868103 PubMed DOI

Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2008). Support for lateralization of the Whorf effect beyond the realm of colour discrimination. Brain and Language, 105(2), 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.06.001 PubMed DOI

Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science, 306(5695), 496–499. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1094492 PubMed DOI

Hansen, T., Olkkonen, M., Walter, S., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). Memory modulates color appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 9(11), 1367–1368. PubMed DOI

Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir–Whorf Hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86(1), 65–79. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050 DOI

Kay, P., & McDaniel, C. K. (1978). The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic colour terms. Language, 54(3), 610–646. https://doi.org/10.2307/412789 DOI

Lindsey, D. T., & Brown, A. M. (2002). Colour naming and the phototoxic effects of sunlight on the eye. Psychological Science, 13(6), 506–512. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00489 PubMed DOI

Lowry, M., & Bryant, J. (2019). Blue is in the eye of the beholder: A cross-linguistic study on color perception and memory. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48, 163–179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9597-0 PubMed DOI

Mackey, A. (2012). Why (or why not), when and how to replicate research. In G. K. Porte (Ed.). Replication research in applied linguistics (1st ed., pp. 34–69). Cambridge University Press.

Regier, T., & Kay, P. (2009). Language, thought, and color: Whorf was half right. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 30(10), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.001 DOI

Roberson, D. & Hanley, J. R. (2010). Relatively speaking: An account of the relationship between language and thought in the colour domain. In B. Malt & P. Wolff (Eds.). Words and the Mind: How words capture human experience (1st ed., pp. 183–198). Oxford University Press.

Shulman, G. L., Pope, D. L. W., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., & Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), 3640–3651. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-09.2010 PubMed DOI

Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. NeuroImage, 14, 76–84. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0839 DOI

Uusküla, M. (2008). Basic colour terms in Finno-Ugric and Slavonic languages: Myths and facts. Doctoral dissertation.

Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on colour discrimination. PNAS, 104(19), 7780–7785. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0701644104 PubMed DOI PMC

Wolff, P., & Holmes, K. J. (2011). Linguistic relativity. Wires Cognitive Science, 2(3), 253–265. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.104 PubMed DOI

Xia, T., Xu, G., & Mo, L. (2018). Bi-lateralized Whorfian effect in colour perception: Evidence from Chinese sign language. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49, 189–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.07.004 DOI

Zhou, K., Mo, L., Kay, P., Kwok, V. P., Ip, T. N., & Tan, L. H. (2010). Newly trained lexical categories produce lateralized categorical perception of color. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(22), 9974–9978. DOI

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...