The Effect of Priming with Photographs of Environmental Settings on Walking Speed in an Outdoor Environment

. 2017 ; 8 () : 73. [epub] 20170126

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid28184208

This study examined the effect of priming with photographs of various environmental settings on the speed of a subsequent outdoor walk in an urban environment. Either photographs of urban greenery, conifer forests, or shopping malls were presented or no prime was employed. Three experiments were conducted (N = 126, N = 88, and N = 121). After being exposed to the priming or no-priming conditions, the participants were asked to walk along an urban route 1.9 km long with vegetation and mature trees (Experiment 1, Experiment 3) or along a route in a modern suburb (Experiment 2). In accord with the concept of approach-avoidance behavior, it was expected that priming with photographs congruent with the environmental setting of the walking route would result in slower walking speed. Conversely, priming with photographs incongruent with the environmental setting should result in faster walking speed. The results showed that priming with the photographs with vegetation caused a decrease in overall walking speed on the route relative to other experimental conditions. However, priming with incongruent primes did not lead to a significant increase in walking speed. In all experimental conditions, the slowest walking speed was found in sections with the highest natural character. The results are explained in terms of congruency between the prime and the environment, as well as by the positive psychological effects of viewing nature.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Aarts H., Dijksterhuis A. (2002). Category activation effects in judgment and behaviour: the moderating role of perceived comparability. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 41 23–138. 10.1348/014466602165090 PubMed DOI

Arnold M. J., Reynolds K. E. (2003). Hedonic shopping motivations. J. Retailing 79 77–95. 10.1016/S0022-4359(03)00007-1 DOI

Ashford K., Jackson R. C. (2010). Priming as a means of preventing skill failure under pressure. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 32 518–536. PubMed

Bargh J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 36 147–168. 10.1002/ejsp.336 PubMed DOI PMC

Bargh J. A., Chen M., Burrows L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71 230–244. 10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230 PubMed DOI

Berman M. G., Jonides J., Kaplan S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19 1207–1212. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x PubMed DOI

Bharucha J. J., Stoeckig K. (1987). Priming of chords: spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? Percept. Psychophys. 41 519–524. 10.3758/BF03210486 PubMed DOI

Bitner M. J. (1992). Servicescapes: the impact of physical surroundings on customers and employees. J. Mark. 56 57–71. 10.2307/1252042 DOI

Bodin M., Hartig T. (2003). Does the outdoor environment matter for psychological restoration gained through running? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 4 141–153. 10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00038-3 DOI

Bornstein M. H., Bornstein H. G. (1976). The pace of life. Nature 259 557–559. 10.1038/259557a0 DOI

Brown D. K., Barton J. L., Gladwell V. F. (2013). Viewing nature scenes positively affects recovery of autonomic function following acute-mental stress. Environ. Sci. Technol. 47 5562–5569. 10.1021/es305019p PubMed DOI PMC

Carter T. J., Ferguson M. J., Hassin R. R. (2011). A single exposure to the American flag shifts support toward Republicanism up to 8 months later. Psychol. Sci. 22 1011–1018. 10.1177/0956797611414726 PubMed DOI

Cesario J., Plaks J. E., Higgins E. T. (2006). Automatic social behavior as motivated preparation to interact. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90 893–910. 10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.893 PubMed DOI

Church B. A., Fisher C. (1998). Long-term auditory word priming in preschoolers: implicit memory support for language acquisition. J. Mem. Lang. 39 523–542. 10.1006/jmla.1998.2601 DOI

Cohen M. J. (ed.) (2003). The Web of Life Imperative. Regenerative Ecopsychology Techniques that Help People Think in Balance with Natural Systems. Friday Harbor, WA: Institute of Global Education.

Crane E., Gross M. (2007). “Motion capture and emotion: affect detection in whole body movement,” in Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction. Lecture Notes in Computer Science Vol. 4738 eds Paiva A. C. R., Prada R., Picard R. W. (Heidelberg: Springer; ), 95–101. 10.1007/978-3-540-74889-2_9 DOI

Custers R., Aarts H. (2010). The unconscious will: how the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science 329 47–50. 10.1126/science.1188595 PubMed DOI

de Kort Y. A. W., Meijnders A. L., Sponselee A. A. G., IJsselsteijn W. A. (2006). What’s wrong with virtual trees? Restoring from stress in a mediated environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 26 309–320. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.09.001 DOI

De Meijer M. (1989). The contribution of general features of body movement to the attribution of emotions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 13 247–268. 10.1007/BF00990296 DOI

Dehaene S., Changeux J. P., Naccache L., Sackur J., Sergent C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10 204–211. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007 PubMed DOI

Dehaene S., Naccache L., Cohen L., Bihan D. L., Mangin J. F., Poline J. B., et al. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat. Neurosci. 4 752–758. 10.1038/89551 PubMed DOI

Dijksterhuis A., Bargh J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33 1–40. 10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80003-4 DOI

Dijksterhuis A., van Knippenberg A. (1998). The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of trivial pursuit. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74 865–877. 10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.865 PubMed DOI

Donovan R. J., Rossiter J. R., Marcoolyn G., Nesdale A. (1994). Store atmosphere and purchasing behavior. J. Retailing 70 283–294. 10.1016/0022-4359(94)90037-X DOI

Doyen S., Klein O., Pichon C.-L., Cleeremans A. (2012). Behavioral priming: it’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS ONE 7:e29081 10.1371/journal.pone.0029081 PubMed DOI PMC

Duvall J. (2011). Enhancing the benefits of outdoor walking with cognitive engagement strategies. J. Environ. Psychol. 31 27–35. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.09.003 DOI

Ekkekakis P., Hall E. E., VanLanduyt L. M., Petruzzello S. J. (2000). Walking in (affective) circles: can short walks enhance affect? J. Behav. Med. 23 245–275. 10.1023/A:1005558025163 PubMed DOI

Ellis L., Abild M., Buker H., Park J. R., He P. (2012). Gendered shopping: a seven country comparison. Mankind Quart. 52 336–357.

Epley N., Gilovich T. (1999). Just going along: nonconscious priming and conformity to social pressure. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 35 578–589. 10.1006/jesp.1999.1390 DOI

Felnhofer A., Kothgassner O. D., Schmidt M., Heinzle A. K., Beutl L., Hlavacs H., et al. (2015). Is virtual reality emotionally arousing? Investigating five emotion inducing virtual park scenarios. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 82 48–56. 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2015.05.004 DOI

Focht B. C. (2009). Brief walks in outdoor and laboratory environments: effects on affective responses, enjoyment, and intentions to walk for exercise. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 80 611–620. 10.1080/02701367.2009.10599600 PubMed DOI

Follenfant A., Légal J.-B., Marie Dit Dinard F., Meyer T. (2005). Effet de l’activation de stéréotypes sur le comportement: une application en contexte sportif. [Effects of stereotype activation on behavior: an application in a sport setting]. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 55 121–129. 10.1016/j.erap.2005.02.002 DOI

Franěk M. (2013). Environmental factors influencing pedestrian walking speed. Percept. Mot. Skills 116 992–1019. 10.2466/06.50.PMS.116.3.992-1019 PubMed DOI

Franěk M., Režný L. (2014). Analýza faktorů ovlivňujících kolísání rychlosti chůze v městském prostředí s přírodními prvky [Analysis of factors affecting variations in walking speed in urban environment with natural elements]. Cesk. Psychol. 58 14–30.

Franěk M., van Noorden L., Režný L. (2014). Tempo and walking speed with music in the urban context. Front. Psychol. 5:1361 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01361 PubMed DOI PMC

Garcia S. M., Weaver K., Moskowitz G. B., Darley J. M. (2002). Crowded minds: the implicit bystander effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83 843–853. 10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.843 PubMed DOI

Harris C. R., Coburn N., Rohrer D., Pashler H. (2013). Two failures to replicate high performance-goal priming effects. PLoS ONE 8:e72467 10.1371/journal.pone.0072467 PubMed DOI PMC

Hartig T., Evans G. W., Jamner L. D., Davis D. S., Garling T. (2003). Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 23 109–123. 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00109-3 DOI

Hartig T., Mang M., Evans G. W. (1991). Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 23 3–26. 10.1177/0013916591231001 DOI

Hartig T., Nyberg L., Nilsson L. G., Gärling T. (1999). Testing for mood congruent recall with environmentally induced mood. J. Environ. Psychol. 19 353–367. 10.1006/jevp.1999.0142 DOI

Herzog T. R., Bosley P. J. (1992). Tranquillity and preference as affective qualities of natural environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 12 115–127. 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80064-7 DOI

Hicheur H., Kadone H., Grezes J., Berthoz A. (2013). “The combined role of motion-related cues and upper body posture for the expression of emotions during human walking,” in Modeling, Simulation and Optimization of Bipedal Walking, Cognitive Systems Monographs Vol. 18 eds Mombaur K., Berns K. (Heidelberg: Springer; ), 71–85. 10.1007/978-3-642-36368-9_6 DOI

Jang S. S., Namkung Y. (2009). Perceived quality, emotions, and behavioral intentions: application of an extended Mehrabian–Russell model to restaurants. J. Bus. Res. 62 451–460. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2008.01.038 DOI

Janiszewski C., Wyer R. S., Jr. (2014). Content and process priming: a review. J. Consum. Psychol. 24 96–118. 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.05.006 DOI

Jiang B., Chang C. Y., Sullivan W. C. (2014). A dose of nature: tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 132 26–36. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.08.005 DOI

Johansson M., Hartig T., Staats H. (2011). Psychological benefits of walking: moderation by company and outdoor environment. Appl. Psychol. 3 261–280. 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2011.01051.x DOI

Kaplan R. (1984). Impact of urban nature: a theoretical analysis. Urban Ecol. 8 189–197. 10.1016/0304-4009(84)90034-2 DOI

Kaplan R., Kaplan S. (1989). The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kristjánsson Á., Campana G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: a review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Atten. Percept. Psychys. 72 5–18. 10.3758/APP.72.1.5 PubMed DOI

Kruger D., Byker D. (2009). Evolved foraging psychology underlies sex differences in shoping experiences and behaviors. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 3 328–342. 10.1037/h0099312 DOI

Kweon B. S., Ulrich R. S., Walker V. D., Tassinary L. G. (2008). Anger and stress the role of landscape posters in an office setting. Environ. Behav. 40 355–381. 10.1177/0013916506298797 DOI

LeBel E. P., Campbell L. (2013). Heightened sensitivity to temperature cues in individuals with high anxious attachment real or elusive phenomenon? Psychol. Sci. 24 2128–2130. 10.1177/0956797613486983 PubMed DOI

Levine R. V., Lynch K., Miyake K., Lucia M. (1989). The Type A city: coronary heart disease and the pace of life. J. Behav. Med. 12 509–524. 10.1007/BF00844822 PubMed DOI

Levine R. V., Norenzayan A. (1999). The pace of life in 31 countries. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 30 178–205. 10.1177/0022022199030002003 DOI

Loersch C., Payne C. B. (2011). The situated inference model: an integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6 234–252. 10.1177/1745691611406921 PubMed DOI

Lottrup L., Stigsdotter U. K., Meilby H., Claudi A. G. (2015). The workplace window view: a determinant of office workers’ work ability and job satisfaction. Landsc. Res. 40 57–75. 10.1080/01426397.2013.829806 DOI

Martin L. L. (1986). Set/reset: use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51 493–504. 10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.493 PubMed DOI

Mayer F. S., Frantz C. M., Bruehlman-Senecal E., Dolliver K. (2009). Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environ. Behav. 41 607–643. 10.1177/0013916508319745 DOI

McMahan E. A., Estes D. (2015). The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 10 507–519. 10.1080/17439760.2014.994224 DOI

Mehrabian A., Russell J. A. (1974). An Approach to Environmental Psychology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Meyer D. E., Schvaneveldt R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. J. Exp. Psychol. 90 227–234. 10.1037/h0031564 PubMed DOI

Michalak J., Troje N. F., Fischer J., Vollmar P., Heidenreich T., Schulte D. (2009). Embodiment of sadness and depression – gait patterns associated with dysphoric mood. Psychosom. Med. 71 580–587. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a2515c PubMed DOI

Milgram S. (1970). Experience of living in cities. Science 167 1461–1468. 10.1126/science.167.3924.1461 PubMed DOI

Montepare J. M., Goldstein S. B., Clausen A. (1987). The identification of emotions from gait information. J. Nonverbal Behav. 11 33–42. 10.1007/BF00999605 DOI

Pisoni D. B., Nusbaum H. C., Luce P. A., Slowiaczek L. M. (1985). Speech perception, word recognition and the structure of the lexicon. Speech Commun. 4 75–95. 10.1016/0167-6393(85)90037-8 PubMed DOI PMC

Ratcliffe E., Gatersleben B., Sowden P. T. (2013). Bird sounds and their contributions to perceived attention restoration and stress recovery. J. Environ. Psychol. 36 221–228. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.08.004 DOI

Roszak T. E., Gomes M. E., Kanner A. D. (eds) (1995). Ecopsychology: Restoring the Earth, Healing the Mind. New York, NY: Sierra Club Books.

Russell J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39 1161–1178. 10.1037/h0077714 DOI

Shariff A. F., Willard A. K., Andersen T., Norenzayan A. (2016). Religious priming: a meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20 27–48. 10.1177/1088868314568811 PubMed DOI

Slowiaczek L. M., McQueen J. M., Soltano E. G., Lynch M. (2000). Phonological representations in prelexical speech processing: evidence from form-based priming. J. Mem. Lang. 43 530–560. 10.1006/jmla.2000.2710 DOI

Staats H., Jahncke H., Herzog T. R., Hartig T. (2016). Urban options for psychological restoration: common strategies in everyday situations. PLoS ONE 11:e0146213 10.1371/journal.pone.0146213 PubMed DOI PMC

Stamps A. E. (1990). Use of photographs to simulate environments: a meta-analysis. Percept. Mot. Skills 71 907–913. 10.2466/pms.1990.71.3.907 PubMed DOI

Steele C. M., Aronson J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69 797–811. 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 PubMed DOI

Tulving E., Schacter D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science 247 301–306. 10.1126/science.2296719 PubMed DOI

Ulrich R. S. (1981). Natural versus urban scenes. Some psychological effects. Environ. Behav. 13 523–556. 10.1177/0013916581135001 DOI

Ulrich R. S. (1986). Human responses to vegetation and landscapes. Landsc. Urban Plan. 13 29–44. 10.1016/0169-2046(86)90005-8 DOI

Ulrich R. S., Simons R. F., Losito B. D., Fiorito E., Miles M. A., Zelson M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11 201–230. 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7 DOI

Ulrich R. S., Simons R. F., Miles M. A. (2003). Effects of environmental simulations and television on blood donor stress. J. Archit. Plan. Res. 20 38–47.

Valtchanov D., Barton K. R., Ellard C. (2010). Restorative effects of virtual nature settings. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 13 503–512. 10.1089/cyber.2009.0308 PubMed DOI

van den Berg A. E., Koole S. L., van der Wulp N. Y. (2003). Environmental preference and restoration: (How) are they related? J. Environ. Psychol. 23 135–146. 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00111-1 DOI

Velarde M. D., Fry G., Tveit M. (2007). Health effects of viewing landscapes–Landscape types in environmental psychology. Urban For. Urban Greening 6 199–212. 10.1016/j.ufug.2007.07.001 DOI

Voigt D., Lindenthal T., Spornberger A. (2007). “Landscape, land use and soul: ecopsychology: mending a troubled relationship,” in Sustainable Food Production and Ethics, eds Zollitsch W., Winckler C., Waiblinger S., Haslberger A. (Wageningen: Academic Publishers; ), 506–511.

Watson D., Clark L. A., Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54 1063–1070. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 PubMed DOI

Wirtz J., Mattila A. S., Tan R. L. P. (2000). The moderating role of target-arousal on the impact of affect on satisfaction—an examination in the context of service experiences. J. Retail. 76 347–365. 10.1016/S0022-4359(00)00031-2 DOI

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

Effect of Traffic Noise and Relaxations Sounds on Pedestrian Walking Speed

. 2018 Apr 14 ; 15 (4) : . [epub] 20180414

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

    Možnosti archivace