Neural Correlates of Liberalism and Conservatism in a Post-communist Country
Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Language English Country Switzerland Media electronic-ecollection
Document type Journal Article
PubMed
31031609
PubMed Central
PMC6474320
DOI
10.3389/fnhum.2019.00119
Knihovny.cz E-resources
- Keywords
- conservatism, error related negativity, liberalism, neuropolitics, political attitude,
- Publication type
- Journal Article MeSH
A previous experiment showed that there was a strong correlation between conservatism/liberalism and brain activity, linked to an error response (r = 0.59, p < 0.001) in the USA political environment. We re-ran the experiment on a larger and age-homogeneous group (n = 100, 50 females and 50 males, aged 20-26 years) in the Czech Republic; a European country with a different sociocultural environment and history. We did not find a relationship between the brain activity connected to conflict monitoring and self-reported conservatism/liberalism orientation (ρ = -0.11, p = 0.297) or conservatism/liberalism validated for the USA agenda (ρ = -0.01, p = 0.910). Instead of replicating the previous study, we decided to test the hypothesis under a different socio-cultural context. Our results support a view of self-reported or validated, conservative or liberal attitudes as a complex behavioral pattern. Such a behavioral pattern cannot be determined with statistical significance, using a simple Go-NoGo detection task, without accounting for confounding factors such as age and socio-cultural conditions. Sufficiently powered studies are warranted to evaluate this neuro-political controversy.
Faculty of Medicine in Hradec Králové Charles University Hradec Králové Czechia
Philosophical Faculty University of Hradec Králové Hradec Králové Czechia
See more in PubMed
Altemeyer B. (1996). The Authoritarian Specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Amodio D. M., Jost J. T., Master S. L., Yee C. M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1246–1247. 10.1038/nn1979 PubMed DOI
Bell D. (2014). What is liberalism? Polit. Theory 42, 682–715. 10.1177/0090591714535103 DOI
Brázdil M., Roman R., Daniel P., Rektor I. (2005). Intracerebral error-related negativity in a simple Go/NoGo task. J. Psychophysiol. 19, 244–255. 10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.244 DOI
Button K. S., Ioannidis J. P. A., Mokrysz C., Nosek B. A., Flint J., Robinson E. S. J., et al. . (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 365–376. 10.1038/nrn3475 PubMed DOI
Buttrick N. R., Oishi S. (2017). The psychological consequences of income inequality. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 11:e12304 10.1111/spc3.12304 DOI
Cichocka A., Bilewicz M., Jost J. T., Marrouch N., Witkowska M. (2016). On the grammar of politics—or why conservatives prefer nouns. Polit. Psychol. 37, 799–815. 10.1111/pops.12327 DOI
Cohen J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 PubMed DOI
Conover P. J., Feldman S. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self-identifications. Am. J. Pol. Sci. 25, 617–645. 10.2307/2110756 DOI
Cornelis I., Van Hiel A., Roets A., Kossowska M. (2009). Age differences in conservatism: evidence on the mediating effects of personality and cognitive style. J. Pers. 77, 51–88. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00538.x PubMed DOI
Danigelis N. L., Hardy M., Cutler S. J. (2007). Population aging, intracohort aging, and sociopolitical attitudes. Am. Sociol. Rev. 72, 812–830. 10.1177/000312240707200508 DOI
Gehring W. J., Liu Y., Orr J. M., Carp J. (2012). “The error-related negativity (ERN/Ne),” in Oxford Library of Psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components, eds Luck S. J., Kappenman E. S. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 231–291.
Gerber A. S., Huber G. A., Doherty D., Dowling C. M., Ha S. E. (2010). Personality and political attitudes: relationships across issue domains and political contexts. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 104, 111–133. 10.1017/s0003055410000031 DOI
Hunter J. D. (1991). Culture Wars: The Struggle to Define America. New York, NY: BasicBooks.
Iannaccone R., Hauser T. U., Staempfli P., Walitza S., Brandeis D., Brem S. (2015). Conflict monitoring and error processing: new insights from simultaneous EEG-fMRI. Neuroimage 105, 395–407. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.028 PubMed DOI
Inglehart R. (1977). The Silent Revolution: Changing Values and Political Styles Among Western Publics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Inzlicht M., McGregor I., Hirsh J. B., Nash K. (2009). Neural markers of religious conviction. Psychol. Sci. 20, 385–392. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02305.x PubMed DOI
Jost J. T. (2009). Elective affinities: on the psychological bases of left-right differences. Psychol. Inq. 20, 129–141. 10.1080/10478400903028599 DOI
Jost J. T., Amodio D. M. (2012). Political ideology as motivated social cognition: behavioral and neuroscientific evidence. Motiv. Emot. 36, 55–64. 10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7 DOI
Jost J. T., Krochik M., Gaucher D., Hennes E. P. (2009). Can a psychological theory of ideological differences explain contextual variability in the contents of political attitudes? Psychol. Inq. 20, 183–188. 10.1080/10478400903088908 DOI
Jost J. T., Nam H. H., Amodio D. M., Van Bavel J. J. (2014). Political neuroscience: the beginning of a beautiful friendship. Polit. Psychol. 35, 3–42. 10.1111/pops.12162 DOI
Jussim L., Crawford J. T., Anglin S. M., Stevens S. T., Duarte J. L. (2016). Interpretations and methods: towards a more effectively self-correcting social psychology. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 66, 116–133. 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.003 DOI
Kluegel J. R., Smith E. R. (2017). Beliefs About Inequality: Americans’ Views of What is and What Ought to Be. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
Larson M. J., Clayson P. E., Clawson A. (2014). Making sense of all the conflict: a theoretical review and critique of conflict-related ERPs. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 283–297. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.007 PubMed DOI
Lass A. (1988). Romantic documents and political monuments: the meaning-fulfillment of history in 19th-century Czech nationalism. Am. Ethnol. 15, 456–471. 10.1525/ae.1988.15.3.02a00030 DOI
Nielsen J. H. (2016). Personality and euroscepticism: the impact of personality on attitudes towards the EU. J. Common Mark. Stud. 54, 1175–1198. 10.1111/jcms.12381 DOI
Nieuwenhuis S., Ridderinkhof K. R., Talsma D., Coles M. G. H., Holroyd C. B., Kok A., et al. . (2002). A computational account of altered error processing in older age: dopamine and the error-related negativity. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 19–36. 10.3758/cabn.2.1.19 PubMed DOI
Olvet D. M., Hajcak G. (2009). The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials. Psychophysiology 46, 957–961. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00848.x PubMed DOI
R Development Core Team (2018). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Found. Stat. Comput. 3503 Available online at: http://www.r-project.org [Accessed April 2, 2019].
Schreiber D. (2017). Neuropolitics: twenty years later. Politics Life Sci. 36, 114–131. 10.1017/pls.2017.25 PubMed DOI
Scruton R. (1980). The Meaning of Conservatism. London: Macmillan.
Skitka L. J., Tetlock P. E. (1993). Providing public assistance: cognitive and motivational processes underlying liberal and conservative policy preferences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 1205–1223. 10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1205 DOI
Soenens B., Duriez B. (2012). Does conservatism have a self-esteem enhancing function? An examination of associations with contingent self-worth and ill-being in late adults. Pers. Individ. Dif. 52, 728–732. 10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.037 DOI
Stanley S. K., Wilson M. S., Milfont T. L. (2017). Exploring short-term longitudinal effects of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation on environmentalism. Pers. Individ. Dif. 108, 174–177. 10.1016/j.paid.2016.11.059 DOI
Tilley J., Evans G. (2014). Ageing and generational effects on vote choice: combining cross-sectional and panel data to estimate APC effects. Elect. Stud. 33, 19–27. 10.1016/j.electstud.2013.06.007 DOI
van Hiel A., Brebels L. (2011). Conservatism is good for you: cultural conservatism protects self-esteem in older adults. Pers. Individ. Dif. 50, 120–123. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.002 DOI
Vittorio Caprara G., Schwartz S., Capanna C., Vecchione M., Barbaranelli C. (2006). Personality and politics: values, traits and political choice. Polit. Psychol. 27, 1–28. 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00457.x DOI
Weissflog M., Choma B. L., Dywan J., van Noordt S. J. R., Segalowitz S. J. (2013). The political (and physiological) divide: political orientation, performance monitoring and the anterior cingulate response. Soc. Neurosci. 8, 434–447. 10.1080/17470919.2013.833549 PubMed DOI
Weissflog M., van Noordt S. J. R., Choma B. L. (2010). Machiavellianism, sociopolitical ideology and the ERN. Psychophysiology 47 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01111.x DOI
Wendell D. G. (2016). Loyola eCommons neural correlates of political attitudes: emotion and ideology in the brain. Available online at: http://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/2299. [Accessed April 2, 2019]
Wilson G. D., Patterson J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 7, 264–269. 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1968.tb00568.x PubMed DOI