Inter-individual differences in laboratory rats as revealed by three behavioural tasks

. 2022 Jun 07 ; 12 (1) : 9361. [epub] 20220607

Jazyk angličtina Země Anglie, Velká Británie Médium electronic

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články, práce podpořená grantem

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid35672428
Odkazy

PubMed 35672428
PubMed Central PMC9174278
DOI 10.1038/s41598-022-13288-w
PII: 10.1038/s41598-022-13288-w
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje

Stable inter-individual differences in behaviour and personality have been studied for several decades now. The aim of this study was to test the repeatability of behaviour of the Long Evans strain of laboratory rats in order to assess their inter-individual differences. Male laboratory rats (n = 36) were tested in a series of tasks (Open field test, Elevated plus maze test, and modified T-maze test) repeated over time to assess their personality traits. To evaluate the temporal stability of the behaviour, we calculated repeatability estimates of the examined traits. We also checked for a link in behavioural traits across these experiments, which would suggest the existence of a behavioural syndrome. We found stable inter-individual differences in behaviour. Interestingly, no link emerged between the tasks we studied and therefore we did not find support for a behavioural syndrome. The lack of behavioural correlations between these experiments suggests that the results derived from these tasks should be interpreted carefully, as these experiments may measure various behavioural axes. Moreover, the animals habituate to the apparatus. Consequently, behaviour in the Open field test and Elevated plus maze test is not fully consistent and repeatable across subsequent trials.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Bell AM, Hankison SJ, Laskowski KL. The repeatability of behaviour: A meta-analysis. Anim. Behav. 2009;77:771–783. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.022. PubMed DOI PMC

Gosling SD. From mice to men: What can we learn about persoanlity from animal research? Psychol. Bull. 2001;127:45–86. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.45. PubMed DOI

Gosling SD, John OP. Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals: A cross-species review. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1999;8:69–75. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00017. DOI

Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol. Rev. 2007;82:291–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x. PubMed DOI

Animal Personalities: Behavior, Physiology, and Evolution. (University of Chicago Press, 2013).

Žampachová B, Landová E, Frynta D. Methods for measuring mammalian personalities: In which animals and how accurately can we quantify it? Lynx. 2017;48:183–198. doi: 10.2478/lynx-2017-0011. DOI

Žampachová B, Kaftanová B, Šimánková H, Landová E, Frynta D. Consistent individual differences in standard exploration tasks in the black rat (Rattus rattus) J. Comp. Psychol. 2017;131:150–162. doi: 10.1037/com0000070. PubMed DOI

Cavigelli SA, Michael KC, Ragan CM. Behavioral, physiological, and health biases in laboratory rodents: A basis for understanding mechanistic links between human personality and health. In: Carere C, Maestripieri D, editors. Animal Personalities: Behavior, Physiology, and Evolution. The University of Chicago Press; 2013. p. 441.

Wang Q, et al. High dose of simvastatin induces hyperlocomotive and anxiolytic-like activities: The association with the up-regulation of NMDA receptor binding in the rat brain. Exp. Neurol. 2009;216:132–138. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.11.016. PubMed DOI

de Oliveira RMW, et al. Expression of neuronal nitric oxide synthase mRNA in stress-related brain areas after restraint in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2000;289:123–126. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01287-8. PubMed DOI

Jessa M, Nazar M, Bidzinski A, Plaznik A. The effects of repeated administration of diazepam, MK-801 and CGP 37849 on rat behavior in two models of anxiety. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1996;6:55–61. doi: 10.1016/0924-977X(95)00068-Z. PubMed DOI

Groothuis TGG, Carere C. Avian personalities: Characterization and epigenesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005;29:137–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010. PubMed DOI

Digman JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1990;41:417–440. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221. DOI

Eysenck HJ. Four ways five factors are not basic. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1992;13:667–673. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90237-J. DOI

Costa PTJ, McCrae RR. Four ways five factors are basic. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1992;13:653–665. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-I. DOI

Réale D, et al. Personality and the emergence of the pace-of-life syndrome concept at the population level. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2010;365:4051–4063. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0208. PubMed DOI PMC

Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. Behavioral syndromes: An ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2004;19:372–378. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.009. PubMed DOI

Biro PA, Stamps JA. Using repeatability to study physiological and behavioural traits: Ignore time-related change at your peril. Anim. Behav. 2015;105:223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.008. DOI

Niemelä PT, Vainikka A, Forsman JT, Loukola OJ, Kortet R. How does variation in the environment and individual cognition explain the existence of consistent behavioral differences? Ecol. Evol. 2012;3:457–464. doi: 10.1002/ece3.451. PubMed DOI PMC

Šimková, O., Frýdlová, P., Žampachová, B., Frynta, D., & Landová, E. Development of behavioural profile in the Northern common boa (Boa imperator): Repeatable independent traits or personality? PLoS ONE12 (2017). PubMed PMC

Stamps JA, Groothuis TGG. Developmental perspectives on personality: Implications for ecological and evolutionary studies of individual differences. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2010;365:4029–4041. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0218. PubMed DOI PMC

Lessells CM, Boag PT. Unrepeatable repeatabilities: A common mistake. Auk. 1987;104:116–121. doi: 10.2307/4087240. DOI

Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: A practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. 2010;85:935–956. PubMed

Perals D, Griffin AS, Bartomeus I, Sol D. Revisiting the open-field test: What does it really tell us about animal personality? Anim. Behav. 2017;123:69–79. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.006. DOI

Denenberg VH. Open field behavior in the rat: What does it mean? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1969;159:852–859. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1969.tb12983.x. PubMed DOI

van der Staay, F. J., Schuurman, T., van Reenen, C. G., & Korte, S. M. Emotional reactivity and cognitive performance in aversively motivated tasks: A comparison between four rat strains. Behav. Brain Funct.5 (2009). PubMed PMC

Ibáñez MI, Ávila C, Ruipérez MA, Moro M, Ortet G. Temperamental traits in mice (I): Factor structure. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2007;43:255–265. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.029. DOI

Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety. Behav. Brain Res. 2000;108:197–203. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00148-5. PubMed DOI

Prut L, Belzung C. The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: A review. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2003;463:3–33. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01272-X. PubMed DOI

Carobrez AP, Bertoglio LJ. Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: The elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005;29:1193–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.017. PubMed DOI

Walsh RN, Cummins RA. The open-field test: A critical review. Psychol. Bull. 1976;83:482–504. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.83.3.482. PubMed DOI

Fernandes C, File SE. The influence of open arm ledges and maze experience in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1996;54:31–40. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02171-X. PubMed DOI

Lalonde R. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2002;26:91–104. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00041-0. PubMed DOI

Torrejais JCM, Rosa CCM, Boerngen-Lacerda R, Andreatini R. The elevated T-maze as a measure of two types of defensive reactions: A factor analysis. Brain Res. Bull. 2008;76:376–379. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.03.016. PubMed DOI

Viana MB, Tomaz C, Graeff FG. The elevated T-maze: A new animal model of anxiety and memory. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behavio. 1994;49:549–554. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90067-1. PubMed DOI

Rodgers RJ, Johnson NJT. Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and ethological measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1995;52:297–303. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00138-M. PubMed DOI

Rodgers RJ, Cao BJ, Dalvi A, Holmes A. Animal models of anxiety: An ethological perspective. Brazilian J. Med. Biol. Res. 1997;30:289–304. doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X1997000300002. PubMed DOI

Castro JE, et al. Personality traits in rats predict vulnerability and resilience to developing stress-induced depression-like behaviors, HPA axis hyper-reactivity and brain changes in pERK1/2 activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1209–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.014. PubMed DOI

Krebs R, Linnenbrink M, Guenther A. Validating standardised personality tests under semi-natural conditions in wild house mice (Mus musculus domesticus) Ethology. 2019;125:761–773. doi: 10.1111/eth.12930. DOI

Rödel HG, Meyer S. Early development influences ontogeny of personality types in young laboratory rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 2011;53:601–613. doi: 10.1002/dev.20522. PubMed DOI

Carola V, D’Olimpio F, Brunamonti E, Mangia F, Renzi P. Evaluation of the elevated plus-maze and open-field tests for the assessment of anxiety-related behaviour in inbred mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;134:49–57. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00452-1. PubMed DOI

Ribeiro A, Ferra-de-Paula V, Pinheiro ML, Palermo-Neto J. Dose-response effects of systemic anandamide administration in mice sequentially submitted to the open field and elevated plus-maze tests. Brazilian J. Med. Biol. Res. 2009;42:556–560. doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X2009000600013. PubMed DOI

Stuchlík, A. et al. Place avoidance tasks as tools in the behavioral neuroscience of learning and memory. Physiol. Res.62 (2013). PubMed

Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Morris water maze: Procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:848–858. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.116. PubMed DOI PMC

D’Hooge, R. & De Deyn, P. P. Applications of the Morris water maze in the study of learning and memory. Brain Res. Rev. 36 (2001). PubMed

Hall CS. Emotional behavior in the rat. I. Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality. J. Comp. Psychol. 1934;18:385–403. doi: 10.1037/h0071444. DOI

Deacon RMJ. Appetitive position discrimination in the T-maze. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:13–15. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.3. PubMed DOI

Locchi F, DallOlio R, Gandolfi O, Rimondini R. Water T-maze, an improved method to assess spatial working memory in rats: Pharmacological validation. Neurosci. Lett. 2007;422:213–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.06.023. PubMed DOI

Zangrossi LIO, Graeff FG. Behavioral validation of the elevated T-maze, a new animal model of anxiety. Brain Res. Bull. 1997;44:1–5. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(96)00381-4. PubMed DOI

Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015;67(1):1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. DOI

R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/ (2020).

RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA. http://www.rstudio.com/ (2020).

Stoffel MA, Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. rptR: Repeatability estimation and variance decomposition by generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2017;8:1639–1644. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12797. DOI

StatSoft Inc. STATISTICA version 9.0., 2009. www.statsoft.com.

IBM Corp. Released 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. IBM Corp.

Hadfield, J. D. 2010 MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: theMCMCglmm R package.J. Stat. Softw.33, 1–22.See http://www.jstatsoft.org/v33/i02/.

Dingemanse NJ, et al. Variation in personality and behavioural plasticity across four populations of the great tit Parus major. J. Anim. Ecol. 2012;81:116–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01877.x. PubMed DOI

Dingemanse NJ, Dochtermann N, Wright J. A method for exploring the structure of behavioural syndromes to allow formal comparison within and between data sets. Anim. Behav. 2010;79:439–450. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.024. DOI

Mutzel A, Dingemanse NJ, Araya-ajoy YG, Kempenaers B. Parental provisioning behaviour plays a key role in linking personality with reproductive success. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2013;280:20131019. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.1019. PubMed DOI PMC

Dingemanse NJ, Dochtermann NA. Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: Mixed-effect modelling approaches. J. Anim. Ecol. 2013 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12013. PubMed DOI

de Boer SF, van der Vegt BJ, Koolhaas JM. Individual variation in aggression of feral rodent strains: A standard for the genetics of aggression and violence? Behav. Genet. 2003;33:485–501. doi: 10.1023/A:1025766415159. PubMed DOI

Modlinska K, Stryjek R, Pisula W. Food neophobia in wild and laboratory rats (multi-strain comparison) Behav. Processes. 2015;113:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.12.005. PubMed DOI

Pisula W, Modlinska K, Chrzanowska A, Stryjek R. Behavioural differences in Brown-Norway and wild-type rats maintained in standard or enriched environment in response to novelty in a familiarised environment. Psychology. 2015;6:251–262. doi: 10.4236/psych.2015.63025. DOI

Stryjek R, Modlińska K, Pisula W. Species specific behavioural patterns (digging and swimming) and reaction to novel objects in wild type, Wistar, Sprague-Dawley and Brown Norway rats. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e40642. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040642. PubMed DOI PMC

Brown GR, Nemes C. The exploratory behaviour of rats in the hole-board apparatus: Is head-dipping a valid measure of neophilia? Behav. Processes. 2008;78:442–448. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.019. PubMed DOI PMC

Ossenkopp K-P, Sorenson L, Mazmanian DS. Factor analysis of open-field behavior in the rat (Rattus norvegicus): Application of the three-way PARAFAC model to a longitudinal data set. Behav. Processes. 1994;31:129–144. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(94)90001-9. PubMed DOI

Poucet B, Durup M, Thinus-Blanc C. Short-term and long-term habituation of exploration in rats, hamsters and gerbils. Behav. Processes. 1988;16:203–211. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(88)90040-X. PubMed DOI

Bell AM, Stamps JA. Development of behavioural differences between individuals and populations of sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Anim. Behav. 2004;68:1339–1348. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.007. DOI

Archer J. Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: A review. Anim. Behav. 1973;21:205–235. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(73)80065-X. PubMed DOI

Martin JGA, Réale D. Temperament, risk assessment and habituation to novelty in eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus. Anim. Behav. 2008;75:309–318. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.026. DOI

Ramos A, Berton O, Mormède P, Chaouloff F. A multiple-test study of anxiety-related behaviours in six inbred rat strains. Behav. Brain Res. 1997;85:57–69. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(96)00164-7. PubMed DOI

Rodgers RJ, Dalvi A. Anxiety, defence and the elevated plus-maze. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1997;21:801–810. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00058-9. PubMed DOI

Vevera J, et al. The effect of prolonged simvastatin application on serotonin uptake, membrane microviscosity and behavioral changes in the animal model. Physiol. Behav. 2016;158:112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.02.029. PubMed DOI

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

How to explore a new environment: exploratory tactics of the black rat (Rattus rattus)

. 2024 Jun ; 70 (3) : 371-382. [epub] 20240404

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...