Facial basis of stereotypes: Judgements of warmth and competence based on cross-group typicality/distinctiveness of faces
Language English Country Great Britain, England Media print-electronic
Document type Journal Article
Grant support
21-10527S
Grantová Agentura České Republiky
PubMed
39347699
PubMed Central
PMC11724689
DOI
10.1111/bjop.12738
Knihovny.cz E-resources
- Keywords
- face perception, facial morphology, geometric morphometrics, intergroup relations, stereotype content model, stereotyping,
- MeSH
- Adult MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Judgment * physiology MeSH
- Adolescent MeSH
- Young Adult MeSH
- Facial Recognition * physiology MeSH
- Social Perception * MeSH
- Stereotyping * MeSH
- Refugees psychology MeSH
- Check Tag
- Adult MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Adolescent MeSH
- Young Adult MeSH
- Male MeSH
- Female MeSH
- Publication type
- Journal Article MeSH
- Geographicals
- Turkey ethnology MeSH
Human migration is an increasingly common phenomenon and migrants are at risk of disadvantageous treatment. We reasoned that migrants may receive differential treatment by locals based on the closeness of their facial features to the host average. Residents of Türkiye, the country with the largest number of refugees currently, served as participants. Because many of these refugees are of Arabic origin, we created target facial stimuli varying along the axis connecting Turkish and Arabic morphological prototypes (excluding skin colour) computed using geometric morphometrics and available databases. Participants made judgements of two universal dimensions of social perception-warmth and competence-on these faces. We predicted that participants judging faces manipulated towards the Turkish average would provide higher warmth and competence ratings compared to judging the same faces manipulated towards the Arabic average. Bayesian statistical tools were employed to estimate parameter values in multilevel models with intercorrelated varying effects. The findings did not support the prediction and revealed raters (as well as target faces) to be an important source of variation in social judgements. In the absence of simple cues (e.g. skin colour, group labels), the effect of facial morphology on social judgements may be much more complex than previously assumed.
Center for Theoretical Study Charles University Prague Czech Republic
See more in PubMed
Abele, A. E. , & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 751–763. 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751 PubMed DOI
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: Isolation and communion in Western man. Beacon Press.
Blair, I. V. , Judd, C. M. , & Chapleau, K. M. (2004). The influence of Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological Science, 15, 674–679. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00739.x PubMed DOI
Blair, I. V. , Judd, C. M. , Sadler, M. S. , & Jenkins, C. (2002). The role of Afrocentric features in person perception: Judging by features and categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 5–25. 10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.5 PubMed DOI
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & Srull T. K. (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. I, pp. 13–48). Psychology Press. 10.4324/9781315801940-6 DOI
Bürhan, P. , & Alici, T. (2023). Enhanced source memory for cheaters with higher resemblance to own‐culture typical faces. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 30, 700–711. 10.3758/s13423-022-02177-y PubMed DOI
Burton, A. M. , Bruce, V. , & Dench, N. (1993). What's the difference between men and women? Evidence from facial measurement. Perception, 22(2), 153–176. 10.1068/p220153 PubMed DOI
Cook, R. , & Over, H. (2021). Why is the literature on first impressions so focused on White faces? Royal Society Open Science, 8(9), 211146. PubMed PMC
Courset, R. , Rougier, M. , Palluel‐Germain, R. , Smeding, A. , Jonte, J. M. , Chauvin, A. , & Muller, D. (2018). The Caucasian and north African French faces (CaNAFF): A face database. International Review of Social Psychology, 31(1), 22. 10.5334/irsp.179 DOI
Cuddy, A. J. , Fiske, S. T. , & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 631–648. PubMed
Eberhardt, J. L. , Davies, P. G. , Purdie‐Vaughns, V. J. , & Johnson, S. L. (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of black defendants predicts capital‐sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 17, 383–386. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01716.x PubMed DOI
Fiske, S. T. , Cuddy, A. J. C. , Glick, P. S. , & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902. PubMed
Foo, Y. Z. , Sutherland, C. A. M. , Burton, N. S. , Nakagawa, S. , & Rhodes, G. (2022). Accuracy in facial trustworthiness impressions: Kernel of truth or modern physiognomy? A meta‐analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(11), 1580–1596. PubMed
Gwinn, O. S. , & Brooks, K. R. (2013). Race‐contingent face aftereffects: A result of perceived racial typicality, not categorization. Journal of Vision, 13(10), 13. 10.1167/13.10.13 PubMed DOI
Haselton, M. G. (2007). Error management theory. In Baumeister R. F. & Vohs K. D. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 311–312). SAGE.
Hebl, M. R. , Williams, M. J. , Sundermann, J. M. , Kell, H. J. , & Davies, P. G. (2012). Selectively friending: Racial stereotypicality and social rejection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1329–1335. 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.05.019 DOI
Hensel, L. B. , Zhan, J. , Bjornsdottir, R. T. , Garrod, O. G. B. , Schyns, P. G. , & Jack, R. E. (2020). Social trait perception is structured by a latent composition of 3D face features. Journal of Vision, 20(11), 1365. 10.1167/jov.20.11.1365 DOI
Hill, H. , Bruce, V. , & Akamatsu, S. (1995). Perceiving the sex and race of faces: The role of shape and colour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 261(1362), 367–373. 10.1098/rspb.1995.0161 PubMed DOI
Imhoff, R. , Woelki, J. , Hanke, S. , & Dotsch, R. (2013). Warmth and competence in your face! Visual encoding of stereotype content. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 386. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00386 PubMed DOI PMC
Jenkins, A. C. , Karashchuk, P. , Zhu, L. , & Hsu, M. (2018). Predicting human behavior toward members of different social groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(39), 9696–9701. PubMed PMC
Kleisner, K. , Pokorný, Š. , & Saribay, S. A. (2019). Towards a new approach to cross‐cultural distinctiveness and typicality of human faces: The cross‐group typicality/distinctiveness metric. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–13. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00124 PubMed DOI PMC
Kleisner, K. , Štěrbová, Z. , & Fiala, V. (2024). What constitutes the perception of facial typicality? Personality and Individual Differences, 231, 112838. 10.1016/j.paid.2024.112838 DOI
Kunst, J. R. , Dovidio, J. F. , & Dotsch, R. (2018). White look‐alikes: Mainstream culture adoption makes immigrants “look” phenotypically white. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(2), 265–282. PubMed
Lewandowski, D. , Kurowicka, D. , & Joe, H. (2009). Generating random correlation matrices based on vines and extended onion method. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 100(9), 1989–2001. 10.1016/j.jmva.2009.04.008 DOI
Liberman, Z. , Woodward, A. L. , & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). The origins of social categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(7), 556–568. 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.004 PubMed DOI PMC
Macrae, C. N. , Milne, A. B. , & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as energy‐saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 37–47. 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.37 DOI
Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 383–401. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_4 PubMed DOI
Maddox, K. B. , Perry, J. M. , & Pagan, J. (2022). Cues and categories: Revisiting paths to racial phenotypicality bias. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 16(8), e12699.
McElreath, R. (2020). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. CRC Press.
Oliveira, M. , Garcia‐Marques, T. , Dotsch, R. , & Garcia‐Marques, L. (2019). Dominance and competence face to face: Dissociations obtained with a reverse correlation approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(5), 888–902.
Rohlf, F. J. (2017). TpsDig. Version 2.30. New York: Department of Ecology and Evolution. State University of New York, Stony Brook NY; 2017.
Rohlf, F. J. , & Marcus, L. F. (1993). A revolution morphometrics. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 8(4), 129–132. PubMed
Russell, R. (2009). A sex difference in facial contrast and its exaggeration by cosmetics. Perception, 38(8), 1211–1219. 10.1068/p6331 PubMed DOI
Saribay, S. A. , Biten, A. F. , Meral, E. O. , Aldan, P. , Třebický, V. , & Kleisner, K. (2018). The Bogazici face database: Standardized photographs of Turkish faces with supporting materials. PLoS One, 13(2), e0192018. PubMed PMC
Sofer, C. , Dotsch, R. , Oikawa, M. , Oikawa, H. , Wigboldus, D. H. , & Todorov, A. (2017). For your local eyes only: Culture‐specific face typicality influences perceptions of trustworthiness. Perception, 46, 914–928. 10.1177/030100661769178 PubMed DOI
Sofer, C. , Dotsch, R. , Wigboldus, D. H. , & Todorov, A. (2015). What is typical is good: The influence of face typicality on perceived trustworthiness. Psychological Science, 26(1), 39–47. 10.1177/0956797614554955 PubMed DOI
Stan Development Team . (2021). Stan Modeling Language Users Guide and Reference Manual v2.27. Retrieved from https://mc‐stan.org
Sutherland, C. A. , Oldmeadow, J. A. , & Young, A. W. (2016). Integrating social and facial models of person perception: Converging and diverging dimensions. Cognition, 157, 257–267. PubMed
Tajfel, H. , & Turner, J. C. (1979). An intergrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin W. G. & Worchel S. (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Brooks/Cole.
Todorov, A. , Said, C. P. , Engell, A. D. , & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(12), 455–460. PubMed
Tracy, R. E. , Wilson, J. P. , Slepian, M. L. , & Young, S. G. (2020). Facial trustworthiness predicts ingroup inclusion decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 91, 104047. 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104047 DOI
Uhlmann, E. L. , Brescoll, V. L. , & Machery, E. (2010). The motives underlying stereotype‐based discrimination against members of stigmatized groups. Social Justice Research, 23(1), 1–16. 10.1007/s11211-010-0110-7 DOI
Wang, X. , Guinote, A. , & Krumhuber, E. G. (2018). Dominance biases in the perception and memory for the faces of powerholders, with consequences for social inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 78, 23–33.
Westfall, J. , Kenny, D. A. , & Judd, C. M. (2014). Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(5), 2020–2045. 10.1037/xge0000014 PubMed DOI
Xie, S. Y. , Flake, J. K. , Stolier, R. M. , Freeman, J. B. , & Hehman, E. (2021). Facial impressions are predicted by the structure of group stereotypes. Psychological Science, 32(12), 1979–1993. 10.1177/09567976211024259 PubMed DOI
Yip, A. W. , & Sinha, P. (2002). Contribution of color to face recognition. Perception, 31(8), 995–1003. 10.1068/p3376 PubMed DOI
Zebrowitz, L. A. , Bronstad, P. M. , & Lee, H. K. (2007). The contribution of face familiarity to ingroup favoritism and stereotyping. Social Cognition, 25(2), 306–338. 10.1521/soco.2007.25.2.306 DOI
Zebrowitz, L. A. , White, B. , & Wieneke, K. (2008). Mere exposure and racial prejudice: Exposure to other‐race faces increases liking for strangers of that race. Social Cognition, 26(3), 259–275. PubMed PMC