Iowa Gambling Task in patients with early-onset Parkinson's disease: strategy analysis
Language English Country Italy Media print-electronic
Document type Journal Article, Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
- MeSH
- Adult MeSH
- Executive Function physiology MeSH
- Gambling psychology MeSH
- Middle Aged MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Neuropsychological Tests standards MeSH
- Parkinson Disease diagnosis epidemiology psychology MeSH
- Psychomotor Performance physiology MeSH
- Decision Making physiology MeSH
- Age of Onset MeSH
- Check Tag
- Adult MeSH
- Middle Aged MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Male MeSH
- Female MeSH
- Publication type
- Journal Article MeSH
- Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't MeSH
The aim of our study was to analyse decision making in early-onset Parkinson's disease (PD) patients performing the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). We compared 19 patients with early-onset PD (≤ 45 years) on dopaminergic medication (no evidence of depression, dementia, executive dysfunction according to the Tower of London test and the Stroop test, or pathological gambling) with 20 age-matched controls. A computer version of the IGT was employed. The PD patients achieved slightly lower IGT scores than the control group. A detailed analysis based on 'shift frequencies' between the individual decks showed that the patients tended to change their preferences for the decks more frequently, with a higher preference for the 'disadvantageous' deck B. Control subjects seemed to develop a more effective strategy. These differences could be caused by the poorer ability of the patients to develop any strategy at all. We observed changes in decision making during IGT performance in patients with early-onset PD, although they had no executive dysfunction as measured by established neuropsychological tests. The more detailed analysis employed in the present study could lead to a more accurate study of IGT performance and application of IGT in clinical practice.
See more in PubMed
Neuropsychologia. 2009 Jul;47(8-9):1901-8 PubMed
Neuroimage. 2005 Jan 1;24(1):253-9 PubMed
Neurol Sci. 2010 Feb;31(1):35-40 PubMed
Eur J Neurosci. 2009 Sep;30(6):1162-71 PubMed
Mov Disord. 2007 Jul 30;22(10):1430-1435 PubMed
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1982 Jun 25;298(1089):199-209 PubMed
J Clin Psychiatry. 1994 Jan;55(1):5-11 PubMed
Neurology. 2006 Jun 13;66(11):1750-2 PubMed
Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Apr;9(4):159-62; discussion 162-4 PubMed
Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2006 Apr;12(3):169-75 PubMed
Mov Disord. 2008 Mar 15;23(4):547-52 PubMed
J Pers. 2009 Jun;77(3):883-902 PubMed
Mov Disord. 2010 Nov 15;25(15):2649-53 PubMed
Neuropsychologia. 2010 Oct;48(12):3377-91 PubMed
Neurology. 1967 May;17(5):427-42 PubMed
Cereb Cortex. 2000 Jun;10(6):552-60 PubMed
Mov Disord. 2010 Jul 30;25(10):1432-6 PubMed
Mov Disord. 2011 Feb 15;26(3):464-8 PubMed
Acta Neurol Belg. 2011 Mar;111(1):3-9 PubMed
Behav Brain Funct. 2007 Mar 15;3:16 PubMed
Clin Psychol Rev. 2010 Jul;30(5):562-81 PubMed
Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(13):2257-67 PubMed
Behav Brain Funct. 2007 Aug 06;3:37 PubMed
Neuropsychol Rev. 2009 Mar;19(1):102-14 PubMed
Mov Disord. 2012 Feb;27(2):331-2 PubMed
Br J Psychiatry. 1979 Apr;134:382-9 PubMed
Neuropsychologia. 2009 Nov;47(13):2882-90 PubMed
Cognition. 1994 Apr-Jun;50(1-3):7-15 PubMed
Adv Neurol. 1990;53:245-9 PubMed
Brain Cogn. 2004 Apr;54(3):240-4 PubMed
Am J Psychiatry. 1987 Sep;144(9):1184-8 PubMed
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2011 Apr;33(4):395-409 PubMed
Behavioral and Neuroanatomical Account of Impulsivity in Parkinson's Disease