Memory for expectation-violating concepts: the effects of agents and cultural familiarity
Language English Country United States Media electronic-ecollection
Document type Journal Article
PubMed
24714568
PubMed Central
PMC3979650
DOI
10.1371/journal.pone.0090684
PII: PONE-D-13-36323
Knihovny.cz E-resources
- MeSH
- Biological Evolution MeSH
- Adult MeSH
- Culture MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Young Adult MeSH
- Memory * MeSH
- Attention MeSH
- Probability MeSH
- Mental Recall MeSH
- Recognition, Psychology MeSH
- Animals MeSH
- Check Tag
- Adult MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Young Adult MeSH
- Male MeSH
- Female MeSH
- Animals MeSH
- Publication type
- Journal Article MeSH
Previous research has shown that ideas which violate our expectations, such as schema-inconsistent concepts, enjoy privileged status in terms of memorability. In our study, memory for concepts that violate cultural (cultural schema-level) expectations (e.g., "illiterate teacher", "wooden bottle", or "thorny grass") versus domain-level (ontological) expectations (e.g., "speaking cat", "jumping maple", or "melting teacher") was examined. Concepts that violate cultural expectations, or counter-schematic, were remembered to a greater extent compared with concepts that violate ontological expectations and with intuitive concepts (e.g., "galloping pony", "drying orchid", or "convertible car"), in both immediate recall, and delayed recognition tests. Importantly, concepts related to agents showed a memory advantage over concepts not pertaining to agents, but this was true only for expectation-violating concepts. Our results imply that intuitive, everyday concepts are equally attractive and memorable regardless of the presence or absence of agents. However, concepts that violate our expectations (cultural-schema or domain-level) are more memorable when pertaining to agents (humans and animals) than to non-agents (plants or objects/artifacts). We conclude that due to their evolutionary salience, cultural ideas which combine expectancy violations and the involvement of an agent are especially memorable and thus have an enhanced probability of being successfully propagated.
See more in PubMed
Barrett JL, Nyhof M (2001) Spreading non-natural concepts. Journal of Cognition & Culture 1: 69–100.
Boyer P, Ramble C (2001) Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science 25: 535–564.
Dawkins R (1976) The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Norenzayan A, Atran S (2004) Cognitive and emotional processes in the cultural transmission of natural and nonnatural beliefs. In Schaller M, Crandall C editors: The psychological foundations of culture, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 149–169.
Norenzayan A, Atran S, Faulkner J, Schaller M (2006) Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science 30: 531–553. PubMed
Sperber D (1996) Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.
Boyer P (1992) Explaining religious ideas: Outline of a cognitive approach. Numen 39: 27–57.
Boyer P (1994) The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Bartlett FC (1932) Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Jacoby LL, Craik FIM (1979) Effects of elaboration of processing at encoding and retrieval: Trace distinctiveness and recovery of initial context. In: Cermak LS, Craik FIM editors: Levels of processing and human memory, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 1–21.
Schmidt SR (1985) Encoding and retrieval processes in the memory for conceptually distinctive events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 11: 565–578. PubMed
Schmidt SR (1991) Can we have a distinct theory for memory? Memory & Cognition 19: 523–542. PubMed
Smith RE, Hunt RR (2000) The effects of distinctiveness require reinstatement of organization: The importance of intentional memory instructions. Journal of Memory & Language 43: 431–446.
Stangor C, McMillan D (1992) Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literatures. Psychological Bulletin 111 ((1)) 42–61.
von Restorff H (1933) Über die Wirkung von Bereichsbildungen im Spurenfeld. Psychologische Forschung 18: 299–342.
Barrett JL (2008) Coding and quantifying counter-intuitiveness in religious concepts: Theoretical and methodological reflections. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 20: 308–338.
Johnson CVM, Kelly SW, Bishop P (2010) Measuring the mnemonic advantage of counter-intuitive and counter-schematic concepts. Journal of Cognition & Culture 10: 109–121.
Purzycki BG (2010) Cognitive architecture, humor and counterintuitiveness: Retention and recall of MCIs. Journal of Cognition & Culture 10 ((1–2)) 189–204.
Purzycki BG (2011) Humor as Violations and Deprecation: A Cognitive Anthropological Account. Journal of Cognition & Culture 10: 189–204.
Kinzler KD, Spelke ES (2007) Core systems in human cognition. Progress in Brain Research 164: 257–264. PubMed
Spelke ES, Kinzler KD (2007) Core knowledge. Developmental Science 10: 89–96. PubMed
Kutas M, Federmeier KD (2000) Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science 4: 463–470. PubMed
Bar M (2009) Predictions: A universal principle in the operation of the human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364 ((1521)) 1181–1182. PubMed PMC
Frith C (2007) Making up the mind: How the brain creates our mental world. Oxford: Blackwell.
Spelke ES (2003) What makes humans smart? Core knowledge and natural language. In Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S editors: Language in mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 149–169.
Boyer P (2001) Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.
Barrett JL (2004) Why would anyone believe in god? Walnut Creek, CA: Alta Mira Press.
Boyer P (2003) Religious thought and behavior as by-products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 119–124. PubMed
Banerjee K, Haque OS, Spelke ES (2014) Melting lizards and crying mailboxes: Children's preferential recall of minimally counterintuitive concepts. Cognitive Science In press. PubMed PMC
Gonce L, Upal MA, Slone DJ, Tweney RD (2006) Role of context in the recall of counterintuitive concepts. Journal of Cognition & Culture 6 ((3–4)) 521–547.
Tweney RD, Upal MA, Gonce LO, Slone DJ, Edwards K (2006) The creative structuring of counterintuitive worlds. Journal of Cognition and Culture 6 ((3–4)) 3–4.
Upal AM (2010) An alternative view of the minimal counterintuitiveness effect. Journal of Cognitive Systems Research 11 ((2)) 194–203.
Upal MA (2011) From individual to social counterintuitiveness: how layers of innovation weave together to form multilayered tapestries of human cultures. Mind & Society 10 ((1)) 79–96.
Upal MA, Gonce LO, Tweney RD, Slone DJ (2007) Contextualizing counterintuitiveness: How context affects comprehension and memorability of counterintuitive concepts. Cognitive Science 31 ((3)) 415–439. PubMed
Harmon-Vukić ME, Slone DJ (2009) The influence of integration on recall for counterintuitive stories. Journal of Cognition and Culture 9 ((1–2)) 57–68.
Libkuman TM, Stabler CL, Otani H (2004) Arousal, valence, and memory for detail. Memory 12: 237–247. PubMed
Fondevila S, Martín-Loeches M, Jiménez-Ortega L, Casado P, Sel A, et al. (2011) The sacred and the absurd—an electrophysiological study of counterintuitive ideas (at sentence level). Social Neuroscience 1: 1–13. PubMed
Harmon-Vukic ME, Upal MA, Sheenan KJ (2012) Understanding the memory advantage of counterintuitive concepts. Brain, Religion, & Behavior 2 ((2)) 121–139.
Scholl BJ, Tremoulet P (2000) Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 ((8)) 299–309. PubMed
Barrett JL (2000) Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 ((1)) 29–34. PubMed
Rapport N, Overing J (2000) Social and cultural anthropology: The key concepts. London: Routledge.
Schank RC (1986) Explanation patterns. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Guthrie S (1993) Faces in the clouds: A New Theory of Religion. Oxford University Press.
Liénard P, Boyer P (2006) Whence collective rituals? A cultural selection model of ritualized behavior. American Anthropologist 108 ((4)) 814–827.
Change LJ, Sanfey AG (2009) Unforgettable ultimatums? Expectation violations promote enhanced social memory following economic bargaining. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 3: 36. PubMed PMC
Hirshman E (1988) The expectation-violation effect: Paradoxical effects of semantic relatedness. Journal of Memory and Language 27: 40–58.
Bartholow BD, Fabiani M, Gratton G, Bettencourt BA (2001) A psychophysiological examination of cognitive processing of and affective responses to social expectancy violations. Psychological Science 12 ((3)) 197–204. PubMed
Bobes MA, Valdes-Sosa M, Olivares E (1994) An ERP study of expectancy violation in face perception. Brain & Cognition 26: 1–22. PubMed
Axmacher N, Cohen MX, Fell J, Haupt S, Dümpelmann M, et al. (2010) Intracranial EEG correlates of expectancy and memory formation in the human hippocampus and nucleus accumbens. Neuron 65: 541–549. PubMed
Boyer P (1998) Cultural Transmission with an Evolved Intuitive Ontology: Domain-Specific Cognitive Tracks of Inheritance. Behavioral & Brain Sciences 21: 570–571.
Onishi KH, Baillargeon R (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308: 255–258. PubMed PMC
Spelke ES, Phillips A, Woodward AL (1995) Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. In: Sperber D, Premack, D, Premack AJ editors: Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 44–78.
Gounden Y, Nicolas S (2012) The impact of processing time on the bizarreness and orthographic distinctiveness effects. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 53: 287–29. PubMed
Brysbaert M, New B (2009) Moving beyond Kucera and Francis: A Critical Evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41: 977–990. PubMed
Slone DJ, Gonce LO, Upal MA, Edwards K, Tweney RD (2007) Imagery effects on recall of minimally counterintuitive concepts. Journal of Cognition and Culture 7: 355–367.