Watching others in a positive state does not induce optimism bias in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), but leads to behaviour indicative of competition
Jazyk angličtina Země Německo Médium print-electronic
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články
Grantová podpora
W 1262
Austrian Science Fund FWF - Austria
DK program W1262-B29
Universität Wien
DK program W1262-B29
Austrian Science Fund
PubMed
33725202
PubMed Central
PMC8360889
DOI
10.1007/s10071-021-01497-1
PII: 10.1007/s10071-021-01497-1
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- Klíčová slova
- Animal emotions, Cognitive bias, Emotional contagion, Judgement bias test, Social cognition,
- MeSH
- Callithrix * MeSH
- emoce * MeSH
- kognice MeSH
- mínění MeSH
- podněty MeSH
- zvířata MeSH
- Check Tag
- zvířata MeSH
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
Emotional contagion is suggested to facilitate group life by enhancing synchronized responses to the environment. Cooperative breeders are an example of a social system that requires such intricate coordination between individuals. Therefore, we studied emotional contagion in common marmosets by means of a judgement bias test. Demonstrators were exposed to an emotion manipulation (i.e., positive, negative, control), and observers perceived only the demonstrator's behaviour. We predicted that the positive or negative states of the demonstrator would induce matching states in the observer, indicating emotional contagion. All subjects' emotional states were assessed through behaviour and cognition, the latter by means of a judgement bias test. Behavioural results showed a successful emotion manipulation of demonstrators, with manipulation-congruent expressions (i.e., positive calls in the positive condition, and negative calls and pilo-erect tail in the negative condition). Observers showed no manipulation-congruent expressions, but showed more scratching and arousal after the positive manipulation. Concerning the judgement bias test, we predicted that subjects in a positive state should increase their response to ambiguous cues (i.e., optimism bias), and subjects in a negative state should decrease their response (i.e., pessimism bias). This prediction was not supported as neither demonstrators nor observers showed such bias in either manipulation. Yet, demonstrators showed an increased response to the near-positive cue, and additional analyses showed unexpected responses to the reference cues, as well as a researcher identity effect. We discuss all results combined, including recently raised validation concerns of the judgement bias test, and inherent challenges to empirically studying emotional contagion.
Bristol Veterinary School Langford House University of Bristol Bristol UK
Department of Zoology University of South Bohemia Budweiss Czech Republic
Evolutionary Cognition Group Department of Anthropology University of Zürich Zürich Switzerland
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Adolphs R, Andler D. Investigating emotions as functional states distinct from feelings. Emot Rev. 2018;10(3):191–201. doi: 10.1177/1754073918765662. PubMed DOI PMC
Adriaense JEC, Koski SE, Huber L, Lamm C. Challenges in the comparative study of empathy and related phenomena in animals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;112:62–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.021. PubMed DOI
Adriaense JEC, Martin JS, Schiestl M, Lamm C, Bugnyar T. Negative emotional contagion and cognitive bias in common ravens (Corvus corax) Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116(23):11547–11552. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1817066116. PubMed DOI PMC
Adriaense JEC, Martin JS, Schiestl M, Lamm C, Bugnyar T (2019b) Reply to Vonk: disentangling emotional contagion from its underlying causes. Proc National Acad Sci 201910556 PubMed PMC
Anderson DJ, Adolphs R. A framework for studying emotions across species. Cell. 2014;157(1):187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.003. PubMed DOI PMC
Ash H, Buchanan-Smith HM. The long-term impact of infant rearing background on the affective state of adult common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2016;174:128–136. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.10.009. PubMed DOI PMC
Aureli F, van Schaik CP. Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) II Coping with the uncertainty. Ethology. 1991;89(2):101–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1991.tb00297.x. DOI
Baciadonna L, Duepjan S, Briefer EF, Padilla de la Torre M, Nawroth C. Looking on the bright side of livestock emotions—the potential of their transmission to promote positive welfare. Front Veter Sci. 2018;5:218. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2018.00218. PubMed DOI PMC
Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM, McKinley J, Smith T. Effects of training on stress-related behavior of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures. J Appl Anim Welfare Sci. 2003;6:221–233. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_07. PubMed DOI
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1):1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. DOI
Bateson M, Desire S, Gartside SE, Wright GA. Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Curr Biol. 2011;21(12):1070–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.017. PubMed DOI PMC
Bateson M, Nettle D. Development of a cognitive bias methodology for measuring low mood in chimpanzees. PeerJ. 2015;3:e998. doi: 10.7717/peerj.998. PubMed DOI PMC
Berthier JM, Semple S. Observing grooming promotes affiliation in Barbary macaques. Proc Royal Soc B. 2018;285(1893):20181964. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.1964. PubMed DOI PMC
Bethell EJ. A “how-to” guide for designing judgment bias studies to assess captive animal welfare. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2015;18(sup1):S18–S42. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2015.1075833. PubMed DOI
Bethell EJ, Holmes A, MacLarnon A, Semple S. Cognitive bias in a non-human primate: husbandry procedures influence cognitive indicators of psychological well-being in captive rhesus macaques. Anim Welf. 2012;21(2):185–195. doi: 10.7120/09627286.21.2.185. DOI
Bezerra BM, Souto A. Structure and usage of the vocal repertoire of Callithrix jacchus. Int J Primatol. 2008;29:671–701. doi: 10.1007/s10764-008-9250-0. DOI
Boissy A, et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol Behav. 2007;92(3):375–397. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003. PubMed DOI
Briefer EF. Vocal contagion of emotions in non-human animals. Proc R Soc B. 2018;285(1873):20172783. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2783. PubMed DOI PMC
Briefer EF, Tettamanti F, McElligott AG. Emotions in goats: mapping physiological, behavioural and vocal profiles. Anim Behav. 2015;99:131–143. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.002. DOI
Brilot BO, Asher L, Bateson M. Stereotyping starlings are more ‘pessimistic’. Anim Cogn. 2010;13(5):721–731. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0323-z. PubMed DOI
Brügger RK, Willems EP, Burkart JM (2021) Do marmosets understand others’ conversations? A thermography approach. Sci Adv 7(6): eabc8790 PubMed PMC
Bučková K, Špinka M, Hintze S. Pair housing makes calves more optimistic. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56798-w. PubMed DOI PMC
Burkart JM, Allon O, Amici F, Fichtel C, Finkenwirth C, et al. The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nat Commun. 2014;5(1):1–9. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5747. PubMed DOI
Burkart JM, Fehr E, Efferson C, van Schaik CP. Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104(50):19762–19766. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710310104. PubMed DOI PMC
Burkart JM, Hrdy SB, van Schaik CP. Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolution Anthropol. 2009;18(5):175–186. doi: 10.1002/evan.20222. DOI
Burkart JM, van Schaik CP. Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? Anim Cogn. 2010;13(1):1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0263-7. PubMed DOI
Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FB, Young LJ. Oxytocin-dependent consolation behaviour in rodents. Science. 2016;351(6271):375–378. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4785. PubMed DOI PMC
Cilia J, Piper DC. Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically-based model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58(1):85–91. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00376-0. PubMed DOI
Clay Z, de Waal FB (2015) Sex and strife: post-conflict sexual contacts in bonobos. In Bonobo Cognition and Behaviour (pp. 67–88). Brill
Clore GL (2018) Emotions: Causes and Consequences. In: Fox AS, Lapate RC, Shackman AJ, Davidson RJ (Eds.) (2018) The nature of emotion: fundamental questions. Oxford University Press
de Waal FB. Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of em- pathy. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. PubMed DOI
De Waal FB. What is an animal emotion? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1224(1):191–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05912.x. PubMed DOI
Decety J. The neural pathways, development and functions of empathy. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;3:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.001. DOI
Dezecache G, Jacob P, Grezes J. Emotional contagion: its scope and limits. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(6):297–299. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.03.011. PubMed DOI
Digby LJ, Barreto CE. Social organization in a wild population of Callithrix jacchus. Folia Primatol. 1993;61(3):123–134. doi: 10.1159/000156739. PubMed DOI
Douglas C, Bateson M, Walsh C, Bédué A, Edwards SA. Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;139(1–2):65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.018. DOI
Edgar JL, Nicol CJ, Clark CCA, Paul ES. Measuring empathic responses in animals. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;138(3–4):182–193. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.006. DOI
Epple G. Comparative studies on vocalization in marmoset monkeys (Hapalidae) Folia Primatol. 1968;8(1):1–40. doi: 10.1159/000155129. PubMed DOI
Epple G. Quantitative studies on scent marking in the marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) Folia Primatol. 1970;13:48–62. doi: 10.1159/000155308. PubMed DOI
Erb WM, Porter LM. Mother's little helpers: What we know (and don't know) about cooperative infant care in callitrichines. Evolution Anthropol. 2017;26(1):25–37. doi: 10.1002/evan.21516. PubMed DOI
Ermatinger FA, Brügger RK, Burkart JM. The use of infrared thermography to investigate emotions in common marmosets. Physiol Behav. 2019;211:112672. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112672. PubMed DOI
Eysenck MW, Mogg K, May J, Richards A, Mathews A. Bias in interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100:144–150. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.144. PubMed DOI
Finkenwirth C, van Schaik C, Ziegler TE, Burkart JM. Strongly bonded family members in common marmosets show synchronized fluctuations in oxytocin. Physiol Behav. 2015;151:246–251. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.07.034. PubMed DOI PMC
Gordon DJ, Rogers LJ. Cognitive bias, hand preference and welfare of common marmosets. Behav Brain Res. 2015;287:100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.037. PubMed DOI
Gygax L. The A to Z of statistics for testing cognitive judgement bias. Anim Behav. 2014;95:59–69. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.06.013. DOI
Harding EJ, Paul ES, Mendl M. Animal behaviour: cognitive bias and affective state. Nature. 2004;427(6972):312. doi: 10.1038/427312a. PubMed DOI
Harrison ML, Tardif SD. Species differences in response to intruders in Callithrix jacchus and Saguinus oedipus. Int J Primatol. 1988;10:343–362. doi: 10.1007/BF02737421. DOI
Hatfield E, Cacioppo JT, Rapson RL. Emotional Contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994.
Hintze S, Melotti L, Colosio S, Bailoo JD, Boada-Saña M, Würbel H, Murphy E. A cross-species judgement bias task: integrating active trial initiation into a spatial Go/No-go task. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23459-3. PubMed DOI PMC
Iigaya K, Jolivald A, Jitkrittum W, Gilchrist ID, Dayan P, Paul E, Mendl M. Cognitive bias in ambiguity judgements: using computational models to dissect the effects of mild mood manipulation in humans. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(11):e0165840. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165840. PubMed DOI PMC
Isern-Mas C, Gomila A. Making sense of emotional contagion. Humana Mente J Philos Stud. 2019;12(35):71–100.
Kato Y, Gokan H, Oh-Nishi A, et al. Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) Behav Brain Res. 2014;275:43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.047. PubMed DOI
Košťál Ľ, Skalná Z, Pichová K (2020) Use of cognitive bias as a welfare tool in poultry. J Animal Sci PubMed PMC
Kramer KL. Cooperative breeding and its significance to the demographic success of humans. Annu Rev Anthropol. 2010;39:417–436. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105054. DOI
Lagisz M, Zidar J, Nakagawa S et al (2020) Optimism, pessimism and judgement bias in animals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev PubMed
Lakin JL, Chartrand TL. Using nonconscious behavioural mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(4):334–339. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14481. PubMed DOI
Lalot M, Ung D, Péron F, d’Ettorre P, Bovet D. You know what? I’m happy. Cognitive bias is not related to personality but is induced by pair-housing in canaries (Serinus canaria) Behav Proc. 2017;134:70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.09.012. PubMed DOI
Lazaro-Perea C. Intergroup interactions in wild common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): Territorial defence and assessment of neighbours. Anim Behav. 2001;62:11–12. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1726. DOI
Lazaro-Perea C, Snowdon CT, de Fátima Arruda M. Scent-marking behavior in wild groups of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1999;46(5):313–324. doi: 10.1007/s002650050625. DOI
MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Bonier F, Romero LM, Moore IT (2019) Glucocorticoids and “stress” are not synonymous. Integrat Organ Biol 1(1): obz017 PubMed PMC
MacLeod AK, Byrne A. Anxiety, depression, and the anticipation of future po- sitive and negative experiences. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105:286–289. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.2.286. PubMed DOI
Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Anim Behav. 1992;44(5):967–979. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80592-5. DOI
Manciocco A, Chiarotti F, Vitale A. Effects of positive interaction with caretakers on the behaviour of socially housed common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;120(1–2):100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.05.007. DOI
Martin JS, Koski SE, Bugnyar T, Jaeggi AV, Massen JJM. Prosociality, social tolerance and partner choice facilitate mutually beneficial cooperation in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus. Anim Behav. 2021;173:115–136. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.12.016. DOI
Martin JS, Massen JJ, Šlipogor V, Bugnyar T, Jaeggi AV, Koski SE. The EGA+ GNM framework: An integrative approach to modelling behavioural syndromes. Methods Ecol Evol. 2019;10(2):245–257. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.13100. DOI
Massen JJ, Behrens F, Martin JS, Stocker M, Brosnan SF (2019) A comparative approach to affect and cooperation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 370–387 PubMed
Massen JJ, Gallup AC. Why contagious yawning does not (yet) equate to empathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80:573–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.006. PubMed DOI
Massen JJ, Šlipogor V, Gallup AC. An Observational investigation of behavioral contagion in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): indications for contagious scent-marking. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01190. PubMed DOI PMC
Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional dis- orders. Annu Rev Psychol. 1994;45(1):25–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.000325. PubMed DOI
McGuire MC, Vonk J. Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) fail to learn abstract cues of differential outcomes in a novel cognitive bias test. Animal Behavior Cogn. 2018;5(1):103–117. doi: 10.26451/abc.05.01.08.2018. DOI
McGuire MC, Vonk J, Fuller G, Allard S. Using an ambiguous cue paradigm to assess cognitive bias in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) during a forage manipulation. Anim Behav Cogn. 2017;4:70–83.
McIntosh DN. Spontaneous facial mimicry, liking and emotional contagion. Polish Psycholog Bull. 2006;37(1):31.
Mendl M, Burman OH, Parker RM, Paul ES. Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;118(3–4):161–181. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023. DOI
Mendl M, Burman OH, Paul ES. An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc R Soc B. 2010;277(1696):2895–2904. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0303. PubMed DOI PMC
Meyza KZ, Bartal IBA, Monfils MH, Panksepp JB, Knapska E. The roots of empathy: through the lens of rodent models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;76:216–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.028. PubMed DOI PMC
Miss FM, Burkart JM. Corepresentation during joint action in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) Psychol Sci. 2018;29(6):984–995. doi: 10.1177/0956797618772046. PubMed DOI
Nakahashi W, Ohtsuki H. Evolution of emotional contagion in group-living animals. J Theor Biol. 2018;440:12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2017.12.015. PubMed DOI
Neal SJ, Caine NG. Scratching under positive and negative arousal in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American journal of primatology, 78(2), 216–226. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;80:573–585. PubMed
Neville V, Nakagawa S, Zidar J, et al. Pharmacological manipulations of judgement bias: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;108:269–286. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.008. PubMed DOI PMC
Nygren TE, Isen AM, Taylor PJ, Dulin J. The influence of positive affect on the decision rule in risky situations. Org Behav Hum Decis Proc. 1996;66:59–72. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0038. DOI
Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: The utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29(3):469–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002. PubMed DOI
Perdue BM. Mechanisms underlying cognitive bias in nonhuman primates. Animal Behavior Cogn. 2017;4(1):105–118. doi: 10.12966/abc.08.02.2017. DOI
Pessoa L (2013) The cognitive-emotional brain: From interactions to integration. MIT press
Péter A (2017) Solomon Coder (version beta 17.03.22): A simple and free solution for behavior coding. Available online at: https://solomon.andraspeter.com/
Pomerantz O, Terkel J, Suomi SJ, Paukner A. Stereotypic head twirls, but not pacing, are related to a ‘pessimistic’-like judgment bias among captive tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) Anim Cogn. 2012;15(4):689–698. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0497-7. PubMed DOI PMC
Preston SD, de Waal FBM. Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci. 2002;25:1–72. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X02000018. PubMed DOI
R Core Team (2019/13) R: A language and environment for statistical computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria
Reimert I, Fong S, Rodenburg TB, Bolhuis JE. Emotional states and emotional contagion in pigs after exposure to a positive and negative treatment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017;193:37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.03.009. DOI
Roelofs S, Boleij H, Nordquist RE, van der Staay FJ. Making decisions under ambiguity: judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016;10:119. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00119. PubMed DOI PMC
Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(6):1161. doi: 10.1037/h0077714. DOI
Russell YI, Phelps S. How do you measure pleasure? A discussion about intrinsic costs and benefits in primate allogrooming. Biol Philos. 2013;28(6):1005–1020. doi: 10.1007/s10539-013-9372-4. DOI
Rygula R, Pluta H, Popik P. Laughing rats are optimistic. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(12):e51959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051959. PubMed DOI PMC
Saito Y, Yuki S, Seki Y, Kagawa H, Okanoya K. Cognitive bias in rats evoked by ultrasonic vocalizations suggests emotional contagion. Behav Proc. 2016;132:5–11. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.08.005. PubMed DOI
Schaffner C, Aureli F. Relationship assessment through emotional mediation. Behaviour. 2002;139(2):393–420. doi: 10.1163/156853902760102726. DOI
Schiel N, Souto A. The common marmoset: An overview of its natural history, ecology and behavior. Devel Neurobio. 2017;77:244–262. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22458. PubMed DOI
Schino G, Aureli F. Reciprocal altruism in primates: partner choice, cognition, and emotions. Adv Study Behav. 2009;39:45–69. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(09)39002-6. DOI
Schino G, Massimei R, Pinzaglia M, Addessi E. Grooming, social rank and ‘optimism’in tufted capuchin monkeys: a study of judgement bias. Anim Behav. 2016;119:11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.06.017. DOI
Schubiger MN, Wüstholz FL, Wunder A, Burkart JM. High emotional reactivity toward an experimenter affects participation, but not performance, in cognitive tests with common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) Anim Cogn. 2015;18(3):701–712. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0837-5. PubMed DOI
Semple S, Harrison C, Lehmann J. Grooming and Anxiety in B arbary Macaques. Ethology. 2013;119(9):779–785. doi: 10.1111/eth.12119. DOI
Šlipogor V, Gunhold-de Oliveira T, Tadić Z, Massen JJM, Bugnyar T. Consistent inter-individual differences in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) in Boldness-Shyness, Stress-Activity and Exploration-Avoidance. Am J Primatol. 2016;78:961–973. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22566. PubMed DOI PMC
Šlipogor V, Massen JJ, Schiel N, Souto A, Bugnyar T (2021) Temporal consistency and ecological validity of personality structure in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): A unifying field and laboratory approach. Am J Primatol e23229. PubMed PMC
Symonds MR, Moussalli A. A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioural ecology using Akaike’s information criterion. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65(1):13–21. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1037-6. DOI
Tardif SD, Richter CB. Competition for a desired food in family groups of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) and the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) Lab Anim Sci. 1981;31(1):52. PubMed
Troisi A. Displacement activities as a behavioral measure of stress in nonhuman primates and human subjects. Stress. 2002;5(1):47–54. doi: 10.1080/102538902900012378. PubMed DOI
Troisi A, Schino G, D'Antoni M, Pandolfi N, Aureli F, D'Amato FR. Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers. Behav Neural Biol. 1991;56(3):307–313. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(91)90469-7. PubMed DOI
Ueno M, Yamada K, Nakamichi M. Emotional states after grooming interactions in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) J Comp Psychol. 2015;129(4):394. doi: 10.1037/a0039688. PubMed DOI
Watson CF, Caldwell CA. Neighbor effects in marmosets: social contagion of agonism and affiliation in captive Callithrix jacchus. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(6):549–558. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20805. PubMed DOI
Yamamoto S, Takimoto A. Empathy and fairness: Psychological mechanisms for eliciting and maintaining prosociality and cooperation in primates. Social Justice Res. 2012;25(3):233–255. doi: 10.1007/s11211-012-0160-0. DOI