The transition from the female-like great calls to male calls during ontogeny in southern yellow-cheeked gibbon males (Nomascus gabriellae)

. 2021 Nov 11 ; 11 (1) : 22040. [epub] 20211111

Jazyk angličtina Země Anglie, Velká Británie Médium electronic

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články, práce podpořená grantem

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid34764403

Grantová podpora
MZE-RO0718 Ministry of Agriculture of the Czech Republic
MZE-RO0718 Ministry of Agriculture of the Czech Republic

Odkazy

PubMed 34764403
PubMed Central PMC8586049
DOI 10.1038/s41598-021-01648-x
PII: 10.1038/s41598-021-01648-x
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje

It is well known that gibbons emit a pattern of vocalizations, which is specific for species and sex. A previous study showed, however, that immature southern yellow-cheeked gibbon (Nomascus gabriellae) males produce only female-like great calls from 2.3 to 5.3 years of age in co-singing interactions with their mothers. To date, nothing is known about how the vocal repertoire of a male changes from the female-like call (great call) to the male call (staccato notes and multi-modulation phrase) during vocal ontogeny. The goal of this study was to describe the transition from the female-like great call to the male call and the ontogeny of the male call. We predicted that the transition from the female-like great call to the male-specific call and the development of the male call is a normal part of the aging proces. If this is the case, the following phenomena will occur: (a) female vocalization should no longer be produced with the mature form of the multi-modulation phrase and (b) all stages of the male vocalization should occur gradually as the young male ages. Young males regularly emit both female-like great calls and male-specific calls between the ages of 5.6 to 7.1 years. Once the young males reached 7.1 years of age, they emitted male calls exclusively, and they continued to do so until the end of the observation period (at 8.11 years of age). It was confirmed that the young males emitted only female-like great calls during periods when they produced non-mature forms of a multi-modulation phrase (Fm0,1-none or one frequency modulation in second notes). Furhermore, the decrease in the number of female-like great calls was attributed to the development of the mature form of the multi-modulation phrase (Fm2-two or more frequency modulation in second notes), which developed with age. We also confirmed that the multi-modulation phrase developed gradually, while the development of the staccato notes occurred in leaps. A multi-modulation phrase developed as the initial part of the male-specific call. It was evolved from a simpler to a more complex form as the maximum frequency and age of the young males increased. Staccato notes subsequently developed in certain young males. Possible explanations for such vocal ontogeny in young males are discussed in this work.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Egnor SER, Hauser MD. A paradox in the evolution of primate vocal learning. Trends. Neurosci. 2004;27:649–654. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.009. PubMed DOI

Zimmermann E. Aspects of reproduction and behavioral and vocal development in Senegal Bushbabies (Galago Senegalensis) Int. J. Primatol. 1989;10:1–16. doi: 10.1007/bf02735700. DOI

Zimmerman E. Loud calls in nocturnal prosimians: Structure, evolution and ontogeny. In: Newman JD, Zimmerman E, Jurgens U, editors. Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication. New York: Plenum Press; 1995. pp. 47–72.

Winter P, Handley P, Ploog D, Schott D. Ontogeny of Squirrel-mokeys calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour. 1973;47:230–0. doi: 10.1163/156853973x00085. PubMed DOI

Hammerschmidt K, Freudenstein T, Jurgens U. Vocal development in squirrel monkeys. Behaviour. 2001;138:1179–1204. doi: 10.1163/156853901753287190. DOI

Elowson AM, Snowdon CT, Sweet CJ. Ontogeny of trill and J-call vocalizations in the pygmy marmoset, cebuella pygmaea. Anim. Behav. 1992;43:703–715. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80195-2. DOI

Pistorio AL, Vintch B, Wang XQ. Acoustic analysis of vocal development in a New World primate, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2006;120:1655–1670. doi: 10.1121/1.2225899. PubMed DOI

Takahashi DY, et al. The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science. 2015;349:734–738. doi: 10.1126/science.aab1058. PubMed DOI

Chevalier-Skolnikoff S. The ontogeny of communication in the stumptail macaque (Macaca arctoides) Contrib. Primatol. 1974;2:1–174.

Gouzoules H, Gouzoules S. Design-features and developmental modification of pigtail macaque, macaca-nemestrina, agonistic screams. Anim. Behav. 1989;37:383–401. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90086-9. DOI

Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Snowdon CT. Affiliative vocalizations in infant rhesus macaques (Macaca-mulatta) J. Comp. Psychol. 1992;106:254–261. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.106.3.254. PubMed DOI

Hammerschmidt K, Newman JD, Champoux M, Suomi SJ. Changes in rhesus macaque 'coo' vocalizations during early development. Ethology. 2000;106:873–886. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00611.x. DOI

Geissmann T. Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biol. Rev. 2002;77:57–76. doi: 10.1017/s1464793101005826. PubMed DOI

Haimoff E. Acoustic and organizational features of gibbon song. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N, editors. The Lesser Apes: Evolutionary and Behavioural Biology. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press; 1984. pp. 333–353.

Konrad R, Geissmann T. Vocal diversity and taxonomy of Nomascus in Cambodia. Int. J. Primatol. 2006;27:713–745. doi: 10.1007/s10764-006-9042-3. DOI

Schilling D. Song bouts and duetting in the concolor gibbon. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N, editors. The Lesser Apes: Evolutionary and Behavioural Biology. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press; 1984. pp. 390–403.

Geissmann T. The yellow-cheeked gibbon (Hylobates-gabriellae) in Nam-Bai-Cat-Tien (Southern Vietnam) revisited. Primates. 1995;36:447–455. doi: 10.1007/bf02382867. DOI

Deputte BL. Duetting in male and female songs of the white-cheeked gibbon (Hylobates concolor leucogenys) In: Snowdon C, Brown C, Petersen M, editors. Primate Communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1982. pp. 67–93.

Koda H, et al. Immature male gibbons produce female-specific songs. Primates. 2014;55:13–17. doi: 10.1007/s10329-013-0390-2. PubMed DOI

Hradec M, Bolechova P, Svobodova I. Production of a female-specific great call in an immature male gibbon, the Nomascus genus. Primates. 2016;57:445–448. doi: 10.1007/s10329-016-0569-4. PubMed DOI

Hradec M, Linhart P, Bartos L, Bolechova P. The traits of the great calls in the juvenile and adolescent gibbon males Nomascus gabriellae. Plos One. 2017 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0173959. PubMed DOI PMC

Burns BL, Dooley HM, Judge DS. Social dynamics modify behavioural development in captive white-cheeked (Nomascus leucogenys) and silvery (Hylobates moloch) gibbons. Primates. 2011;52:271–277. doi: 10.1007/s10329-011-0247-5. PubMed DOI

Burns BL, Judge DS. The varied path to adulthood: Plasticity in developmental timing in hylobatids. Am. J. Primatol. 2016;78:610–625. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22523. PubMed DOI

Koda H, et al. Possible role of mother–daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in Gibbons. Plos One. 2013 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071432. PubMed DOI PMC

Brockelman WY, Reichard U, Treesucon U, Raemaekers JJ. Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons (Hylobates lar) Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1998;42:329–339. doi: 10.1007/s002650050445. DOI

Reichard UH. Social monogamy in gibbons: The male perspective. In: Boesch C, Reichard UH, editors. Monogamy: Mating Strategies and Partnerships in Birds, Humans and Other Mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003. pp. 190–213.

Brockelman WY, Schilling D. Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls. Nature. 1984;312:634–636. doi: 10.1038/312634a0. PubMed DOI

Geissmann T. Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analyzed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus × Hylobates lar) Folia. Primatol. 1984;42:216–235. doi: 10.1159/000156165. DOI

Cowlishaw G. Song function in gibbons. Behaviour. 1992;121:131–153. doi: 10.1163/156853992X00471. DOI

Geissmann T, Orgeldinger M. The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs, Hylobates syndactylus. Anim. Behav. 2000;60:805–809. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1540. PubMed DOI

Leighton DR. Gibbons territoriality and monorgamy. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT, editors. Primates Societies. Chicago: Chicago University Press; 1987. pp. 135–145.

Koda H, et al. Soprano singing in gibbons. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2012;149:347–355. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22124. PubMed DOI

Huang MP, Ma HG, Ma CY, Garber PA, Fan PF. Male gibbon loud morning calls conform to Zipf's law of brevity and Menzerath's law: Insights into the origin of human language. Anim. Behav. 2020;160:145–155. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.11.017. DOI

Podos J. A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes:Emberizidae) Evolution. 1997;51:537–551. doi: 10.2307/2411126. PubMed DOI

Illes AE, Hall ML, Vehrencamp SL. Vocal performance influences male receiver response in the banded wren. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2006;273:1907–1912. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3535. PubMed DOI PMC

de Kort SR, Eldermire ERB, Valderrama S, Botero CA, Vehrencamp SL. Trill consistency is an age-related assessment signal in banded wrens. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2009;276:2315–2321. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0127. PubMed DOI PMC

Pasch B, George AS, Campbell P, Phelps SM. Androgen-dependent male vocal performance influences female preference in Neotropical singing mice. Anim. Behav. 2011;82:177–183. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.04.018. DOI

Gil D, Gahr M. The honesty of bird song: Multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2002;17:133–141. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(02)02410-2. DOI

Barelli C, Mundry R, Heistermann M, Hammerschmidt K. Cues to Androgens and Quality in Male Gibbon Songs. Plos One. 2013 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082748. PubMed DOI PMC

Newman SR, Butler J, Hammond EH, Gray SD. Preliminary report on hormone receptors in the human vocal fold. J. Voice. 2000;14:72–81. doi: 10.1016/s0892-1997(00)80096-x. PubMed DOI

Kirzinger A, Jurgens U. Role of extralaryngeal muscles in phonation of subhuman primates. J. Comp. Physiol. A. Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 1994;175:215–222. doi: 10.1007/bf00215117. PubMed DOI

Snowdon CT, Elowson AM. 'Babbling' in pygmy marmosets: Development after infancy. Behaviour. 2001;138:1235–1248. doi: 10.1163/15685390152822193. DOI

Bouchet H, Blois-Heulin C, Lemasson A. Age- and sex-specific patterns of vocal behavior in De Brazza's monkeys (Cercopithecus neglectus) Am. J. Primatol. 2012;74:12–28. doi: 10.1002/ajp.21002. PubMed DOI

Kojima S. Early vocal development in a chimpanzee infant. In: Matsuzawa T, editor. Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior. Tokyo: Springer; 2001. pp. 190–198.

Laporte MNC, Zuberbuhler K. The development of a greeting signal in wild chimpanzees. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:1220–1234. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01069.x. PubMed DOI

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...