• This record comes from PubMed

A synthesis of evidence for policy from behavioural science during COVID-19

. 2024 Jan ; 625 (7993) : 134-147. [epub] 20231213

Language English Country Great Britain, England Media print-electronic

Document type Journal Article

Links

PubMed 38093007
PubMed Central PMC10764287
DOI 10.1038/s41586-023-06840-9
PII: 10.1038/s41586-023-06840-9
Knihovny.cz E-resources

Scientific evidence regularly guides policy decisions1, with behavioural science increasingly part of this process2. In April 2020, an influential paper3 proposed 19 policy recommendations ('claims') detailing how evidence from behavioural science could contribute to efforts to reduce impacts and end the COVID-19 pandemic. Here we assess 747 pandemic-related research articles that empirically investigated those claims. We report the scale of evidence and whether evidence supports them to indicate applicability for policymaking. Two independent teams, involving 72 reviewers, found evidence for 18 of 19 claims, with both teams finding evidence supporting 16 (89%) of those 18 claims. The strongest evidence supported claims that anticipated culture, polarization and misinformation would be associated with policy effectiveness. Claims suggesting trusted leaders and positive social norms increased adherence to behavioural interventions also had strong empirical support, as did appealing to social consensus or bipartisan agreement. Targeted language in messaging yielded mixed effects and there were no effects for highlighting individual benefits or protecting others. No available evidence existed to assess any distinct differences in effects between using the terms 'physical distancing' and 'social distancing'. Analysis of 463 papers containing data showed generally large samples; 418 involved human participants with a mean of 16,848 (median of 1,699). That statistical power underscored improved suitability of behavioural science research for informing policy decisions. Furthermore, by implementing a standardized approach to evidence selection and synthesis, we amplify broader implications for advancing scientific evidence in policy formulation and prioritization.

274th ASOS US Air Force New York Air National Guard Syracuse NY United States

Center for Adaptive Rationality Max Planck Institute for Human Development Berlin Germany

Center for Science Communication Research School of Journalism and Communication University of Oregon Eugene OR USA

Center for Social Norms and Behavioral Dynamics University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia PA USA

Centre for Applied Epistemology Educational Research Institute Ljubljana Slovenia

CESifo Munich Germany

Charles University Prague Czech Republic

City University of London London UK

Computation and Neural Systems Program California Institute of Technology Pasadena CA USA

Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas Buenos Aires Argentina

Cornell University Ithaca NY USA

Cowry Consulting London UK

Department of Agricultural and Resource Economics University of Maryland College Park MD USA

Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology Faculty of Psychology University of Vienna Vienna Austria

Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford UK

Department of General Experimental Developmental and Health Psychology Sofia University St Kliment Ohridski Sofia Bulgaria

Department of Health Policy and Management Columbia University Mailman School of Public Health New York City NY USA

Department of Humanities and Social Sciences California Institute of Technology Pasadena CA USA

Department of Politics and Center for Social Media and Politics New York University New York NY USA

Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science London School of Economics London UK

Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science New York University New York NY USA

Department of Psychology and Laboratory for Research of Individual Differences Faculty of Philosophy University of Belgrade Belgrade Serbia

Department of Psychology Carleton University Ottawa Ontario Canada

Department of Psychology Faculty of Arts University of Ljubljana Ljubljana Slovenia

Department of Psychology Humboldt University of Berlin Berlin Germany

Department of Psychology Loyola University Chicago Chicago IL USA

Department of Psychology Northwestern University Evanston IL USA

Department of Psychology Ohio State University Columbus OH USA

Department of Psychology Stanford University Stanford CA USA

Department of Psychology The Chinese University of Hong Kong Hong Kong SAR China

Department of Psychology University of British Columbia Vancouver British Columbia Canada

Department of Psychology University of Cambridge Cambridge UK

Department of Psychology University of Cologne Cologne Germany

Department of Psychology University of Zurich Zurich Switzerland

Department of Psychology York University Toronto Ontario Canada

Department of Social Policy and Evaluation University of Oxford Oxford UK

Department of Sociology Stanford University Stanford CA USA

Duke Kunshan University Kunshan China

Environmental Psychology Department of Cognition Emotion and Methods in Psychology Faculty of Psychology University of Vienna Vienna Austria

Escuela de Negocios Universidad Torcuato Di Tella Buenos Aires Argentina

Faculty of Arts and Science Kyushu University Fukuoka Japan

Global Faculty Social and Economic Behavior University of Cologne Cologne Germany

Gordon Institute of Business Science University of Pretoria Johannesburg South Africa

Haas School of Business University of California Berkeley Berkeley CA USA

Harvard University Cambridge MA USA

Institute for Brain and Behavior Amsterdam Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam Amsterdam Netherlands

Institute of Psychology Faculty of Philosophy University of Belgrade Belgrade Serbia

Jacobs Center for Productive Youth Development University of Zurich Zurich Switzerland

Junior Researcher Programme Cambridge UK

Kellogg School of Management Northwestern University Evanston IL USA

Laboratorio de Neurociencia Universidad Torcuato Di Tella Buenos Aires Argentina

Lehigh University Bethlehem PA USA

Mackenzie Presbyterian University São Paulo Brazil

Mannheim Centre for European Social Research University of Mannheim Mannheim Germany

Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge MA USA

MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit University of Cambridge Cambridge UK

National Institute of Science and Technology on Social and Affective Neuroscience CNPq São Paulo Brazil

National University of Singapore Singapore Singapore

Northwestern University Evanston IL USA

Organisation for Economic Co operation and Development Paris France

Policy Research Group Centre for Business Research Judge Business School University of Cambridge Cambridge UK

Psychology Department Faculty of Health and Life Sciences University of Exeter Exeter UK

Psychology Department University of Oregon Eugene OR USA

Rotman Research Institute Baycrest Academy for Research and Education Toronto Ontario Canada

Stanford University Stanford CA USA

Sungkyunkwan University Seoul Republic of Korea

The Wharton School of the University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia PA USA

The World Bank Washington DC USA

Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience Trinity College Dublin Dublin Ireland

University College Dublin Dublin Ireland

University College Oxford UK

University of Amsterdam Amsterdam Netherlands

University of Bonn University Hospital Bonn Institute of General Practice and Family Medicine Bonn Germany

University of Chicago Chicago IL USA

University of Groningen Groningen Netherlands

University of Illinois Chicago Chicago IL USA

University of Kent Canterbury UK

University of Michigan Ann Arbor MI USA

University of Milan Bicocca Milan Italy

University of Oxford Oxford UK

University of Queensland St Lucia Queensland Australia

University of Tehran Tehran Iran

University of Texas at Austin Austin TX USA

Utrecht University Utrecht Netherlands

See more in PubMed

National Academies Press. Using Science as Evidence in Public Policy (National Academies Press, 2012).

Fact sheet: new progress on using behavioral science insights to better serve the American people. whitehouse.govhttps://obamawhitehouse.archives.gov/the-press-office/2016/09/15/fact-sheet-new-progress-using-behavioral-science-insights-better-serve (2016).

Van Bavel JJ, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z. PubMed DOI

Hodges R, Caperchione E, van Helden J, Reichard C, Sorrentino D. The role of scientific expertise in COVID-19 policy-making: evidence from four European countries. Public Org. Rev. 2022;22:249–267. doi: 10.1007/s11115-022-00614-z. DOI

Dowd JB, et al. Demographic science aids in understanding the spread and fatality rates of COVID-19. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:9696–9698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2004911117. PubMed DOI PMC

Parkhurst JO, Abeysinghe S. What constitutes “good” evidence for public health and social policy-making? From hierarchies to appropriateness. Soc. Epistemol. 2016;30:665–679. doi: 10.1080/02691728.2016.1172365. DOI

Sienkiewicz, M. & Mair, D. in Science for Policy Handbook (eds Šucha, V. & Sienkiewicz, M.) 2–13 (Elsevier, 2020).

Manski CF. Communicating uncertainty in policy analysis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2019;116:7634–7641. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1722389115. PubMed DOI PMC

Moatti J-P. The French response to COVID-19: intrinsic difficulties at the interface of science, public health, and policy. Lancet Public Health. 2020;5:e255. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30087-6. PubMed DOI PMC

Tyler C, et al. AI tools as science policy advisers? The potential and the pitfalls. Nature. 2023;622:27–30. doi: 10.1038/d41586-023-02999-3. PubMed DOI

Policy citations for ‘using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response’ Altmetric (accessed 22 November 2023) https://nature.altmetric.com/details/80902343/policy-documents.

Cheng C, Barceló J, Hartnett AS, Kubinec R, Messerschmidt L. COVID-19 government response event dataset (CoronaNet v.1.0) Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:756–768. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0909-7. PubMed DOI

World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe. Pandemic fatigue: reinvigorating the public to prevent COVID-19: policy framework for supporting pandemic prevention and management: revised version November 2020. WHOhttps://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/337574 (2020).

European Commission, Directorate-General for Research and Innovation, European Group on Ethics in Science and New Technologies & Group of Chief Scientific Advisors. Improving pandemic preparedness and management: lessons learned and ways forward: independent expert report. Publications Office of the European Unionhttp://op.europa.eu/en/publication-detail/-/publication/a1016d77-2562-11eb-9d7e-01aa75ed71a1/language-en/format-PDF (2020).

Biddle, N. Behavioural economics and the COVID-induced education crisis. OECDhttps://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/behavioural-economics-and-the-covid-induced-education-crisis_eba2b867-en (2021).

Principles for managing SARS-CoV-2 transmission associated with further education, 3 September 2020. GOV.UKhttps://www.gov.uk/government/publications/principles-for-managing-sars-cov-2-transmission-associated-with-further-education-3-september-2020 (2020).

Diminishing returns: nudging COVID-19 prevention among Colombian young adults. IDBhttps://publications.iadb.org/en/diminishing-returns-nudging-covid-19-prevention-among-colombian-young-adults (2021). PubMed PMC

Duckett, S., Mackey, W., Stobart, A. & Swerissen, H. Coming out of COVID-19 lockdown: the next steps for Australian health care. APOhttps://apo.org.au/node/306385 (2020).

Social scientists scramble to study pandemic, in real time. Sciencehttps://www.science.org/content/article/social-scientists-scramble-study-pandemic-real-time (2020).

What are behavioral scientists doing about COVID-19? The Policy Lab (accessed 22 November 2023) https://thepolicylab.brown.edu/projects/covid-behavioral-tracker.

Van Lange, P. A. M., & Rand, D. G. Human cooperation and the crises of climate change, COVID-19, and misinformation. Annu. Rev. Psychol.73, 379–402 (2022). PubMed

Byrne-Davis LMT, et al. Using behavioural science in public health settings during the COVID-19 pandemic: the experience of public health practitioners and behavioural scientists. Acta Psychol. 2022;224:103527. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103527. PubMed DOI PMC

Ioannidis JPA, Salholz-Hillel M, Boyack KW, Baas J. The rapid, massive growth of COVID-19 authors in the scientific literature. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021;8:210389. doi: 10.1098/rsos.210389. PubMed DOI PMC

IJzerman H, et al. Use caution when applying behavioural science to policy. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:1092–1094. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00990-w. PubMed DOI

Bryan CJ, Tipton E, Yeager DS. Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:980–989. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01143-3. PubMed DOI PMC

Lewis NA, Wai J. Communicating what we know and what isn’t so: science communication in psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021;16:1242–1254. doi: 10.1177/1745691620964062. PubMed DOI

Puthillam, A. Too WEIRD, too fast: preprints about COVID-19 in the psychological sciences. Collabra Psychol.10.1525/collabra.74331 (2023).

Galluccio, M. in Science and Diplomacy: Negotiating Essential Alliances (ed. Galluccio, M.) 65–74 (Springer International Publishing, 2021).

Cairney P, Oliver K. Evidence-based policymaking is not like evidence-based medicine, so how far should you go to bridge the divide between evidence and policy? Health Res. Policy Syst. 2017;15:35. doi: 10.1186/s12961-017-0192-x. PubMed DOI PMC

Ellemers N. Science as collaborative knowledge generation. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2021;60:1–28. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12430. PubMed DOI PMC

Ruppel Shell, E. Act now, wait for perfect evidence later, says ‘high priestess’ of U.K. COVID-19 masking campaign. Science10.1126/science.abf2811 (2020).

Pei S, Kandula S, Shaman J. Differential effects of intervention timing on COVID-19 spread in the United States. Sci. Adv. 2020;6:eabd6370. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abd6370. PubMed DOI PMC

Schwartz JL. Evaluating and deploying Covid-19 vaccines — the importance of transparency, scientific integrity, and public trust. N. Engl. J. Med. 2020;383:1703–1705. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp2026393. PubMed DOI

Philipp-Muller A, Lee SWS, Petty RE. Why are people antiscience, and what can we do about it? Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2022;119:e2120755119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2120755119. PubMed DOI PMC

Wingen T, Berkessel JB, Englich B. No replication, no trust? How low replicability influences trust in psychology. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2020;11:454–463. doi: 10.1177/1948550619877412. DOI

Kojan L, Burbach L, Ziefle M, Calero Valdez A. Perceptions of behaviour efficacy, not perceptions of threat, are drivers of COVID-19 protective behaviour in Germany. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2022;9:1–15. doi: 10.1057/s41599-022-01098-4. DOI

Santana AP, Korn L, Betsch C, Böhm R. Lessons learned about willingness to adopt various protective measures during the early COVID-19 pandemic in three countries. PLoS ONE. 2022;17:e0265892. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0265892. PubMed DOI PMC

Garcia-Garzon, E., Angulo-Brunet, A., Lecuona, O., Barrada, J. R. & Corradi, G. Exploring COVID-19 research credibility among Spanish scientists. Curr. Psychol.10.1007/s12144-022-02797-6 (2022). PubMed PMC

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. How to build COVID-19 vaccine confidence. CDChttps://www.cdc.gov/vaccines/covid-19/vaccinate-with-confidence/building-trust.html (2022).

Nosek BA, et al. Replicability, robustness, and reproducibility in psychological science. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2022;73:719–748. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-114157. PubMed DOI

Serra-Garcia M, Gneezy U. Mistakes, overconfidence, and the effect of sharing on detecting lies. Am. Econ. Rev. 2021;111:3160–3183. doi: 10.1257/aer.20191295. DOI

Haber NA, Wieten SE, Smith ER, Nunan D. Much ado about something: a response to “COVID-19: underpowered randomised trials, or no randomised trials?”. Trials. 2021;22:780. doi: 10.1186/s13063-021-05755-y. PubMed DOI PMC

‘It’s misinformation at worst.’ Weak health studies can do more harm than good, scientists say. Sciencehttps://www.science.org/content/article/it-s-misinformation-worst-weak-health-studies-can-do-more-harm-good-scientists-say (2021).

Luttrell A, Petty RE. Evaluations of self-focused versus other-focused arguments for social distancing: an extension of moral matching effects. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2021;12:946–954. doi: 10.1177/1948550620947853. DOI

Korn L, Böhm R, Betsch C. Reply to Rabb et al.: why promoting COVID-19 vaccines with community immunity is not a good strategy (yet) Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2102054118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2102054118. PubMed DOI PMC

Van Bavel JJ, et al. National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:517. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-27668-9. PubMed DOI PMC

Frenzel SB, et al. How national leaders keep ‘us’ safe: a longitudinal, four-nation study exploring the role of identity leadership as a predictor of adherence to COVID-19 non-pharmaceutical interventions. BMJ Open. 2022;12:e054980. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-054980. PubMed DOI PMC

Lu JG, Jin P, English AS. Collectivism predicts mask use during COVID-19. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2021793118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2021793118. PubMed DOI PMC

Stevenson C, Wakefield JRH, Felsner I, Drury J, Costa S. Collectively coping with coronavirus: local community identification predicts giving support and lockdown adherence during the COVID-19 pandemic. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2021;60:1403–1418. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12457. PubMed DOI PMC

Bentley, S. V. et al. GROUPS 2 CONNECT: an online activity to maintain social connection and well-being during COVID-19. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being10.1111/aphw.12330 (2022). PubMed

Bicchieri C, et al. In science we (should) trust: expectations and compliance across nine countries during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0252892. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252892. PubMed DOI PMC

Hensel L, et al. Global behaviors, perceptions, and the emergence of social norms at the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Econ. Behav. Org. 2022;193:473–496. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2021.11.015. PubMed DOI PMC

Chadwick A, et al. Online social endorsement and Covid-19 vaccine hesitancy in the United Kingdom. Soc. Media Soc. 2021;7:20563051211008816.

Rabb N, Bowers J, Glick D, Wilson KH, Yokum D. The influence of social norms varies with “others” groups: evidence from COVID-19 vaccination intentions. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2022;119:e2118770119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2118770119. PubMed DOI PMC

Stuart J, O’Donnell K, O’Donnell A, Scott R, Barber B. Online social connection as a buffer of health anxiety and isolation during COVID-19. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2021;24:521–525. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2020.0645. PubMed DOI

Marinucci M, Pancani L, Aureli N, Riva P. Online social connections as surrogates of face-to-face interactions: a longitudinal study under Covid-19 isolation. Comp. Hum. Behav. 2022;128:107102. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107102. DOI

Sun, R., Rieble, C., Liu, Y. & Sauter, D. Connected despite lockdown: the role of social interactions and social media use in wellbeing. Collabra: Psychol.8, 37061 (2022).

Tunçgenç B, et al. Social influence matters: We follow pandemic guidelines most when our close circle does. Br. J. Psychol. 2021;112:763–780. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12491. PubMed DOI PMC

van Baal, S. T., Walasek, L. & Hohwy, J. Modelling pandemic behaviour using an economic multiplayer game. Sci. Rep.12, 13466 (2022). PubMed PMC

Zhu P, et al. The efficacy of a brief, altruism-eliciting video intervention in enhancing COVID-19 vaccination intentions among a population-based sample of younger adults: randomized controlled trial. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2022;8:e37328. doi: 10.2196/37328. PubMed DOI PMC

Abu-Akel A, Spitz A, West R. The effect of spokesperson attribution on public health message sharing during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0245100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245100. PubMed DOI PMC

Arroyos-Calvera, D., Drouvelis, M., Lohse, J. & McDonald, R. Improving compliance with COVID-19 guidance: a workplace field experiment. SSRN10.2139/ssrn.3760562 (2021).

Bos B, Drupp MA, Meya JN, Quaas MF. Moral suasion and the private provision of public goods: evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic. Environ. Res. Econ. 2020;76:1117–1138. doi: 10.1007/s10640-020-00477-2. PubMed DOI PMC

Chu J, Pink SL, Willer R. Religious identity cues increase vaccination intentions and trust in medical experts among American Christians. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2106481118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2106481118. PubMed DOI PMC

Larsen, B. et al. Counter-stereotypical messaging and partisan cues: moving the needle on vaccines in a polarized U.S. http://www.nber.org/papers/w29896.pdf (NBER, 2022). PubMed PMC

Moehring, A. et al. Surfacing norms to increase vaccine acceptance. SSRN10.2139/ssrn.3782082 (2021).

Rabb N, et al. Evidence from a statewide vaccination RCT shows the limits of nudges. Nature. 2022;604:E1–E7. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04526-2. PubMed DOI

Teeny JD, Siev JJ, Briñol P, Petty RE. A review and conceptual framework for understanding personalized matching effects in persuasion. J. Consum. Psychol. 2021;31:382–414. doi: 10.1002/jcpy.1198. DOI

Flores A, et al. Politicians polarize and experts depolarize public support for COVID-19 management policies across countries. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2022;119:e2117543119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2117543119. PubMed DOI PMC

Long A, et al. Co-created messaging for influenza vaccination in a high-risk Hispanic community provides groundwork for COVID-19 vaccine. Health Equity. 2021;5:345–352. doi: 10.1089/heq.2020.0132. PubMed DOI PMC

de Saint Laurent C, Murphy G, Hegarty K, Greene CM. Measuring the effects of misinformation exposure and beliefs on behavioural intentions: a COVID-19 vaccination study. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2022;7:87. doi: 10.1186/s41235-022-00437-y. PubMed DOI PMC

Su Z, et al. Mental health consequences of COVID-19 media coverage: the need for effective crisis communication practices. Global. Health. 2021;17:4. doi: 10.1186/s12992-020-00654-4. PubMed DOI PMC

Loomba S, de Figueiredo A, Piatek SJ, de Graaf K, Larson HJ. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:337–348. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01056-1. PubMed DOI

Druckman JN, Klar S, Krupnikov Y, Levendusky M, Ryan JB. Affective polarization, local contexts and public opinion in America. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:28–38. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-01012-5. PubMed DOI

Farhart CE, Douglas-Durham E, Lunz Trujillo K, Vitriol JA. Vax attacks: how conspiracy theory belief undermines vaccine support. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 2022;188:135–169. doi: 10.1016/bs.pmbts.2021.11.001. PubMed DOI PMC

Freeman, D. et al. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: the Oxford coronavirus explanations, attitudes, and narratives survey (Oceans) II. Psychol. Med.10.1017/S0033291720005188 (2020). PubMed PMC

Gelfand, M. J. et al. The relationship between cultural tightness–looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: a global analysis. Lancet Planet. Health10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30301-6 (2021). PubMed PMC

Gollwitzer, A., Martel, C., Brady, W. J., Knowles, E. D. & Van Bavel, J. Partisan differences in physical distancing predict infections and mortality during the coronavirus pandemic. SSRN10.2139/ssrn.3609392 (2020).

Kitayama S, Camp NP, Salvador CE. Culture and the COVID-19 pandemic: multiple mechanisms and policy implications. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2022;16:164–211. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12080. DOI

Kreps S, Dasgupta N, Brownstein JS, Hswen Y, Kriner DL. Public attitudes toward COVID-19 vaccination: the role of vaccine attributes, incentives, and misinformation. npj Vaccines. 2021;6:73. doi: 10.1038/s41541-021-00335-2. PubMed DOI PMC

Marchlewska M, Hamer K, Baran M, Górska P, Kaniasty K. COVID-19: why do people refuse vaccination? The role of social identities and conspiracy beliefs: evidence from nationwide samples of Polish adults. Vaccines. 2022;10:268. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10020268. PubMed DOI PMC

Painter, M. & Qiu, T. Political beliefs affect compliance with government mandates. J. Econ. Behav. Org.10.1016/j.jebo.2021.03.019 (2021). PubMed PMC

Porter, E., Wood, T. J. & Velez, Y. Correcting COVID-19 vaccine misinformation in ten countries. R. Soc. Open Sci.10, 221097 (2023). PubMed PMC

Rodriguez, C. G., Gadarian, S. K., Goodman, S. W. & Pepinsky, T. B. Morbid polarization: exposure to COVID-19 and partisan disagreement about pandemic response. Polit. Psychol.10.1111/pops.12810 (2022). PubMed PMC

Roozenbeek J, et al. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020;7:201199. doi: 10.1098/rsos.201199. PubMed DOI PMC

Basol M, et al. Towards psychological herd immunity: cross-cultural evidence for two prebunking interventions against COVID-19 misinformation. Big Data Soc. 2021;8:20539517211013868. doi: 10.1177/20539517211013868. DOI

Pennycook G, Rand DG. Accuracy prompts are a replicable and generalizable approach for reducing the spread of misinformation. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:2333. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30073-5. PubMed DOI PMC

Capraro, V. & Celadin, T. “I think this news is accurate”: endorsing accuracy decreases the sharing of fake news and increases the sharing of real news. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.10.1177/01461672221117691 (2022). PubMed PMC

Elgar FJ, Stefaniak A, Wohl MJA. The trouble with trust: time-series analysis of social capital, income inequality, and COVID-19 deaths in 84 countries. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020;263:113365. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113365. PubMed DOI PMC

Khazanchi, R., Marcelin, J., Abdul-Mutakabbir, J. & Essien, U. Race, racism, civil rights law, and the equitable allocation of scarce COVID-19 treatments. Health Aff. Forefront10.1377/forefront.20220208.453850 (2022).

Scheitle CP, Corcoran KE. COVID-19 skepticism in relation to other forms of science skepticism. Socius. 2021;7:23780231211049840. doi: 10.1177/23780231211049841. DOI

Milkman, K. L. et al. A citywide experiment testing the impact of geographically targeted, high-pay-off vaccine lotteries. Nat. Hum. Behav.10.1038/s41562-022-01437-0 (2022). PubMed

Campos-Mercade P, et al. Monetary incentives increase COVID-19 vaccinations. Science. 2021;374:879–882. doi: 10.1126/science.abm0475. PubMed DOI PMC

Thirumurthy H, Milkman KL, Volpp KG, Buttenheim AM, Pope DG. Association between statewide financial incentive programs and COVID-19 vaccination rates. PLoS ONE. 2022;17:e0263425. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0263425. PubMed DOI PMC

Gulati K, Busari J. Vaccinating a billion people against COVID-19: India’s quest for systems leadership in exceptional times. Leadersh. Health Serv. 2021;35:137–148. doi: 10.1108/LHS-05-2021-0045. PubMed DOI

Cheema S, Ameduri M, Abraham A, Doraiswamy S, Mamtani R. The COVID-19 pandemic: the public health reality. Epidemiol. Infect. 2020;148:e223. doi: 10.1017/S0950268820002216. PubMed DOI PMC

European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies et al. Public health leadership and the COVID-19 pandemic in Europe. Eurohealth. 2021;27:4–9.

Nkengasong J, et al. The global response to the COVID-19 pandemic. Med. 2020;1:3–8. doi: 10.1016/j.medj.2020.12.003. PubMed DOI PMC

Schwalb A, Seas C. The COVID-19 pandemic in Peru: what went wrong? Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2021;104:1176–1178. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.20-1323. PubMed DOI PMC

Ortega F, Orsini M. Governing COVID-19 without government in Brazil: ignorance, neoliberal authoritarianism, and the collapse of public health leadership. Glob. Public Health. 2020;15:1257–1277. doi: 10.1080/17441692.2020.1795223. PubMed DOI

Brownstein, J., Cantor, J. H., Rader, B., Simon, K. I. & Whaley, C. M. If you build it, will they vaccinate? The impact of COVID-19 vaccine sites on vaccination rates and outcomes. 10.3386/w30429 (NBER, 2022).

Thunström L, Ashworth M, Shogren JF, Newbold S, Finnoff D. Testing for COVID-19: willful ignorance or selfless behavior? Behav. Public Policy. 2021;5:135–152. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2020.15. DOI

Leonhardt JM, Ridinger G, Rong Y, Talaei-Khoe A. Invincibility threatens vaccination intentions during a pandemic. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0258432. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258432. PubMed DOI PMC

Dai H, et al. Behavioural nudges increase COVID-19 vaccinations. Nature. 2021;597:404–409. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03843-2. PubMed DOI PMC

Reñosa MDC, et al. Nudging toward vaccination: a systematic review. BMJ Glob. Health. 2021;6:e006237. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2021-006237. PubMed DOI PMC

Epstein, Z. et al. Developing an accuracy-prompt toolkit to reduce COVID-19 misinformation online. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Rev.10.37016/mr-2020-71 (2021).

Roozenbeek J, Freeman ALJ, van der Linden S. How accurate are accuracy-nudge interventions? A preregistered direct replication of Pennycook et al. (2020) Psychol. Sci. 2021;32:1169–1178. doi: 10.1177/09567976211024535. PubMed DOI PMC

Maier M, et al. No evidence for nudging after adjusting for publication bias. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2022;119:e2200300119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2200300119. PubMed DOI PMC

DellaVigna S, Linos E. RCTs to scale: comprehensive evidence from two nudge units. Econometrica. 2022;90:81–116. doi: 10.3982/ECTA18709. DOI

Aguilar-Latorre A, et al. The impact of the COVID-19 lockdown on depression sufferers: a qualitative study from the province of Zaragoza, Spain. BMC Public Health. 2022;22:780. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-13083-2. PubMed DOI PMC

Loades ME, et al. Rapid systematic review: the impact of social isolation and loneliness on the mental health of children and adolescents in the context of COVID-19. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2020;59:1218–1239.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.05.009. PubMed DOI PMC

Pancani L, Marinucci M, Aureli, N, Riva P. Forced social isolation and mental health: a study on 1,006 Italians under COVID-19 lockdown. Front. Psychol. 2021;12:663799. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663799. PubMed DOI PMC

World Health Organization. Mental health and COVID-19: early evidence of the pandemic’s impact: scientific brief, 2 March 2022. WHOwww.who.int/publications-detail-redirect/WHO-2019-nCoV-Sci_Brief-Mental_health-2022.1 (2022).

Sels L, et al. Intimate relationships in times of COVID-19: a descriptive study of Belgian partners and their perceived well-being. Psychol. Belg. 2022;62:1–16. doi: 10.5334/pb.1088. PubMed DOI PMC

Boserup B, McKenney M, Elkbuli A. Alarming trends in US domestic violence during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. J. Emerg. Med. 2020;38:2753–2755. doi: 10.1016/j.ajem.2020.04.077. PubMed DOI PMC

Measuring the Shadow Pandemic Violence Against Women During COVID-19 (United Nations, 2021).

Zion SR, et al. Making sense of a pandemic: mindsets influence emotions, behaviors, health, and wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc. Sci. Med. 2022;301:114889. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114889. PubMed DOI PMC

Ruggeri K, Garcia-Garzon E, Maguire Á, Matz S, Huppert FA. Well-being is more than happiness and life satisfaction: a multidimensional analysis of 21 countries. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 2020;18:192. doi: 10.1186/s12955-020-01423-y. PubMed DOI PMC

Fieselmann J, Annac K, Erdsiek F, Yilmaz-Aslan Y, Brzoska P. What are the reasons for refusing a COVID-19 vaccine? A qualitative analysis of social media in Germany. BMC Public Health. 2022;22:846. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-13265-y. PubMed DOI PMC

Bullock J, Lane JE, Shults FL. What causes COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy? Ignorance and the lack of bliss in the United Kingdom. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2022;9:1–7. doi: 10.1057/s41599-022-01092-w. DOI

Schumpe BM, et al. Predictors of adherence to public health behaviors for fighting COVID-19 derived from longitudinal data. Sci. Rep. 2022;12:3824. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-04703-9. PubMed DOI PMC

Myerson J, Strube MJ, Green L, Hale S. Individual differences in COVID-19 mitigation behaviors: the roles of age, gender, psychological state, and financial status. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0257658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0257658. PubMed DOI PMC

Belchior, A. M. & Teixeira, C. P. Determinants of political trust during the early months of the COVID-19 pandemic: putting policy performance into evidence. Polit. Stud. Rev.10.1177/14789299211056193 (2021). PubMed PMC

Viner R, et al. School closures during social lockdown and mental health, health behaviors, and well-being among children and adolescents during the first COVID-19 wave: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatr. 2022;176:400–409. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.5840. PubMed DOI

Baumann M. COVID-19 and mental health in children and adolescents: a diagnostic panel to map psycho-social consequences in the pandemic context. Discov. Ment. Health. 2021;1:2. doi: 10.1007/s44192-021-00002-x. PubMed DOI PMC

Gómez-Durán EL, et al. COVID-19 pandemic psychological impact and volunteering experience perceptions of medical students after 2 years. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2022;19:7532. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19127532. PubMed DOI PMC

Jiang X, Hwang J, Shah DV, Ghosh S, Brauer M. News attention and social-distancing behavior amid COVID-19: how media trust and social norms moderate a mediated relationship. Health Commun. 2022;37:768–777. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2020.1868064. PubMed DOI PMC

Fields, J. et al. Community–academic partnerships to address Covid-19 inequities: lessons from the San Francisco Bay Area. NEJM Catalyst10.1056/CAT.21.0135 (2021).

Mullin M. Learning from local government research partnerships in a fragmented political setting. Public Adm. Rev. 2021;81:978–982. doi: 10.1111/puar.13395. DOI

DeBoer S, et al. Building successful and sustainable academic health science partnerships: exploring perspectives of hospital leaders. Can. Med. Educ. J. 2019;10:e56–e67. doi: 10.36834/cmej.53013. PubMed DOI PMC

Baker, S. R., Farrokhnia, R. A., Meyer, S., Pagel, M. & Yannelis, C. Income, liquidity, and the consumption response to the 2020 Economic Stimulus Payments. NBERhttps://www.nber.org/papers/w27097 (2020).

D'Agostino, E. M. et al. School–academic partnerships in support of safe return to schools during the COVID-19 pandemic. Pediatrics10.1542/peds.2021-054268C (2022). PubMed PMC

Ruggeri K, et al. Role of military forces in the New York State response to COVID-19. JAMA Health Forum. 2022;3:e222136. doi: 10.1001/jamahealthforum.2022.2136. PubMed DOI

Subbaraman N. Science misinformation alarms Francis Collins as he leaves top NIH job. Nature. 2021;600:372–373. doi: 10.1038/d41586-021-03611-2. PubMed DOI

Habersaat KB, et al. Ten considerations for effectively managing the COVID-19 transition. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:677–687. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0906-x. PubMed DOI

Haug N, et al. Ranking the effectiveness of worldwide COVID-19 government interventions. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:1303–1312. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-01009-0. PubMed DOI

Wang Z, Nayfeh T, Tetzlaff J, O’Blenis P, Murad MH. Error rates of human reviewers during abstract screening in systematic reviews. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0227742. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227742. PubMed DOI PMC

Guterman EL, Braunstein LZ. Preprints during the COVID-19 pandemic: public health emergencies and medical literature. J. Hosp. Med. 2020;15:634–636. doi: 10.12788/jhm.3491. PubMed DOI PMC

Wingen, T., Berkessel, J. B. & Dohle, S. Caution, preprint! Brief explanations allow nonscientists to differentiate between preprints and peer-reviewed journal articles. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci.10.1177/25152459211070559 (2022).

Ruggeri, K. Psychology and Behavioral Economics: Applications for Public Policy (Routledge, 2022).

Ruggeri, K. (ed.). Behavioral Insights for Public Policy: Concepts and Cases (Routledge, 2019).

Ruggeri, K., Benzerga, A., Verra, S. & Folke, T. A behavioral approach to personalizing public health. Behav. Public Policy10.1017/bpp.2020.31 (2020).

Ruggeri K, et al. Replicating patterns of prospect theory for decision under risk. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:622–633. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0886-x. PubMed DOI

Ruggeri, K. et al. The general fault in our fault lines. Nat. Hum. Behav.10.1038/s41562-021-01092-x (2021). PubMed

Ruggeri, K. et al. The globalizability of temporal discounting. Nat. Hum. Behav.10.1038/s41562-022-01392-w (2022). PubMed PMC

Ruggeri, K. et al. Standards for evidence in policy decision-making. Social Sciencessocialsciences.nature.com/users/399005-kai-ruggeri/posts/standards-for-evidence-in-policy-decision-making (2020).

Leek JT, Peng RD. Statistics: P values are just the tip of the iceberg. Nature. 2015;520:612. doi: 10.1038/520612a. PubMed DOI

Altman N, Krzywinski M. Interpreting P values. Nat. Methods. 2017;14:213–214. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.4210. DOI

Van Bavel JJ, Mende-Siedlecki P, Brady WJ, Reinero DA. Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:6454–6459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521897113. PubMed DOI PMC

Find record

Citation metrics

Loading data ...

Archiving options

Loading data ...