Disgust as a primary emotional system and its clinical relevance

. 2024 ; 15 () : 1454774. [epub] 20240828

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid39295749

This paper advocates for considering disgust as a primary emotional system within Panksepp's Affective Neuroscience framework, which has the potential to improve the efficacy of psychotherapy with obsessive-compulsive disorder, hypochondriasis, and emetophobia. In 2007, Toronchuk and Ellis provided comprehensive evidence that DISGUST system, as they defined it, matched all Panksepp's criteria for a primary emotional system. A debate ensued and was not unambiguously resolved. This paper is an attempt to resume this discussion and supplement it with the data that accumulated since then on DISGUST's relationship with the immune system and the role of DISGUST dysregulation in psychopathology. We hope that renewed research interest in DISGUST has the potential to improve clinical efficacy with hard-to-treat conditions.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Adolphs R. (2017). How should neuroscience study emotions? By distinguishing emotion states, concepts, and experiences. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 24–31. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw153, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Ainley V., Apps M. A., Fotopoulou A., Tsakiris M. (2016). ‘Bodily precision’: a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160003. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0003, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Alcaro A., Panksepp J. (2011). The SEEKING mind: primal neuro-affective substrates for appetitive incentive states and their pathological dynamics in addictions and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1805–1820. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.002, PMID: PubMed DOI

Athey A. J., Elias J. A., Crosby J. M., Jenike M. A., Pope H. G., Jr., Hudson J. I., et al. . (2015). Reduced disgust propensity is associated with improvement in contamination/washing symptoms in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Obs. Comp. Related Disorders 4, 20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.11.001, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Bhat A., Parr T., Ramstead M., Friston K. (2021). Immunoceptive inference: why are psychiatric disorders and immune responses intertwined? Biol. Philos. 36:27. doi: 10.1007/s10539-021-09801-6, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Bloch M., Pittenger C. (2010). The genetics of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Curr. Psychiatr. Rev. 6, 91–103. doi: 10.2174/157340010791196439, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Bongard J., Zykov V., Lipson H. (2006). Resilient machines through continuous self-modeling. Science 314, 1118–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.1133687, PMID: PubMed DOI

Brake C. A., Tipsword J. M., Badour C. L. (2021). Mental contamination, disgust, and other negative emotions among survivors of sexual trauma: results from a daily monitoring study. J. Anxiety Disord. 84:102477. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102477, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Calder A. J., Keane J., Manes F., Antoun N., Young A. W. (2000). Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1077–1078. doi: 10.1038/80586, PMID: PubMed DOI

Cervin M., Perrin S. (2021). Incompleteness and disgust predict treatment outcome in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 52, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2020.01.007, PMID: PubMed DOI

Chapman H. A., Anderson A. K. (2012). Understanding disgust. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1251, 62–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06369.x PubMed DOI

Chapman H. A., Anderson A. K. (2013). Things rank and gross in nature: a review and synthesis of moral disgust. Psychol. Bull. 139, 300–327. doi: 10.1037/a0030964, PMID: PubMed DOI

Chen W. G., Schloesser D., Arensdorf A. M., Simmons J. M., Cui C., Valentino R., et al. . (2021). The emerging science of interoception: sensing, integrating, interpreting, and regulating signals within the self. Trends Neurosci. 44, 3–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.10.007, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Ciaunica A., Levin M., Rosas F., Friston K. (2023b). Nested selves: self-organisation and shared Markov blankets in prenatal development in humans. Topics Sci, 1–23. doi: 10.1111/tops.12717 PubMed DOI

Ciaunica A., Shmeleva E. V., Levin M. (2023a). The brain is not mental! Coupling neuronal and immune cellular processing in human organisms. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 17:1057622. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2023.1057622, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Craig A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894 PubMed DOI

Curtis V., Aunger R., Rabie T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 271, S131–S133. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Davey G. C., Bond N. (2006). Using controlled comparisons in disgust psychopathology research: the case of disgust, hypochondriasis and health anxiety. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatr. 37, 4–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2005.09.001 PubMed DOI

de Silva P., Marks M. (1999). The role of traumatic experiences in the genesis of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 37, 941–951. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00185-5, PMID: PubMed DOI

Dlouhá D., Roberts S. C., Hlaváčová J., Nouzová K., Kaňková Š. (2023). Longitudinal changes in disgust sensitivity during pregnancy and the early postpartum period, and the role of recent health problems. Sci. Rep. 13:4752. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-31060-6, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Dykshoorn K. L. (2014). Trauma-related obsessive–compulsive disorder: a review. Health Psychol. Behav. Med. Open Access J. 2, 517–528. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2014.905207, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Ekman P., Friesen W. V. (1986). A new pan-cultural facial expression of emotion. Motiv. Emot. 10, 159–168. doi: 10.1007/BF00992253 DOI

Endres D., Pollak T. A., Bechter K., Denzel D., Pitsch K., Nickel K., et al. . (2022). Immunological causes of obsessive-compulsive disorder: is it time for the concept of an “autoimmune OCD” subtype? Transl. Psychiatry 12:5. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01700-4, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Evrard H. C. (2019). The organization of the primate insular cortex. Front. Neuroanat. 13:43. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2019.00043, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Fejzo M., Rocha N., Cimino I., Lockhart S. M., Petry C. J., Kay R. G., et al. . (2023). GDF15 linked to maternal risk of nausea and vomiting during pregnancy. Nature 625, 760–767. doi: 10.1038/s41586-023-06921-9, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Fessler D., Eng S., Navarrete C. (2005). Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy – evidence supporting prophylaxis the compensatory hypothesis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 344–351. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.12.001 DOI

Fessler D., Navarrete C. (2003). Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 406–417. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00054-0 DOI

Fields C., Bischof J., Levin M. (2020). Morphological coordination: a common ancestral function unifying neural and non-neural signaling. Physiology 35, 16–30. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00027.2019, PMID: PubMed DOI

Fields C., Levin M. (2020). Scale-free biology: integrating evolutionary and developmental thinking. Bio Essays 42:e1900228. doi: 10.1002/bies.201900228, PMID: PubMed DOI

Fields C., Levin M. (2022). Competency in navigating arbitrary spaces as an invariant for analyzing cognition in diverse embodiments. Entropy 24:819. doi: 10.3390/e24060819 PubMed DOI PMC

Fields C., Levin M. (2023). Regulative development as a model for origin of life and artificial life studies. Biosystems 229:104927. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2023.104927, PMID: PubMed DOI

Fleischman D. S., Fessler D. M. (2011). Progesterone's effects on the psychology of disease avoidance: support for the compensatory behavioral prophylaxis hypothesis. Horm. Behav. 59, 271–275. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.11.014, PMID: PubMed DOI

Fleischman D. S., Fessler D. M. T. (2018). Response to “hormonal correlates of pathogen disgust: testing the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis”. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 468–469. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.006 DOI

Fotopoulou A., Tsakiris M. (2017). Mentalizing homeostasis: the social origins of interoceptive inference. Neuropsychoanalysis 19, 3–28. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2017.1294031 DOI

Friston K. (2013). Life as we know it. J. R. Soc. Interface 10:20130475. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2013.0475, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Friston K., Levin M., Sengupta B., Pezzulo G. (2015). Knowing one's place: a free-energy approach to pattern regulation. J. R. Soc. Interface 12:20141383. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2014.1383, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Friston K. J., Wiese W., Hobson J. A. (2020). Sentience and the origins of consciousness: from Cartesian duality to Markovian monism. Entropy 22:516. doi: 10.3390/e22050516, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Gan X., Zhou X., Li J., Jiao G., Jiang X., Biswal B., et al. . (2022). Common and distinct neurofunctional representations of core and social disgust in the brain: coordinate-based and network meta-analyses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 135:104553. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104553, PMID: PubMed DOI

Geenen V. (2021). The thymus and the science of self. Seminars Immunopathol. 43, 5–14. doi: 10.1007/s00281-020-00831-y PubMed DOI PMC

Gelberg H. (2018). Pathophysiological mechanisms of gastrointestinal toxicity. Compr. Toxicol. 21, 139–178. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-801238-3.10923-7 DOI

Green B. (2013). Post-traumatic stress disorder: new directions in pharmacotherapy. Adv. Psychiatr. Treat. 19, 181–190. doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.111.010041 DOI

Haidt J., McCauley C., Rozin P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 16, 701–713. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7 DOI

Halley F. M. (1991). Self-regulation of the immune system through biobehavioral strategies. Biofeedback Self Regul. 16, 55–74. doi: 10.1007/BF01000446, PMID: PubMed DOI

Hoehl S., Hellmer K., Johansson M., Gredebäck G. (2017). Itsy bitsy spider…: infants react with increased arousal to spiders and snakes. Front. Psychol. 8:1710. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01710, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Holtmann O., Bruchmann M., Mönig C., Schwindt W., Melzer N., Miltner W. H. R., et al. . (2020). Lateralized deficits of disgust processing after insula-basal ganglia damage. Front. Psychol. 11:1429. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01429, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Inui T., Sugishita T., Inui-Yamamoto C., Yasoshima Y., Shimura T. (2019). The basolateral nucleus of the amygdala executes the parallel processes of avoidance and palatability in the retrieval of conditioned taste aversion in male rats. Eneuro 6:ENEURO.0004. doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0004-19.2019 PubMed DOI PMC

Jabbi M., Bastiaansen J., Keysers C. (2008). A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways. PLoS One 3:e2939. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002939, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Jackson M., Solms M. (2013). Separation distress in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychoanalysis 15, 117–125. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2013.10799825 DOI

Jones B. C., Hahn A. C., Fisher C. I., Wang H., Kandrik M., Lee A. J., et al. . (2018). Hormonal correlates of pathogen disgust: testing the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 166–169. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.004 DOI

Joyce M. K. P., Marshall L. G., Banik S. L., Wang J., Xiao D., Bunce J. G., et al. . (2021). Pathways for memory, cognition and emotional context: hippocampal, Subgenual area 25, and Amygdalar axons show unique interactions in the primate thalamic Reuniens nucleus. J. Neurosci. 42, 1068–1089. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1724-21.2021, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Kaňková Š., Hlaváčová J., Roberts K., Benešová J., Havlíček J., Calda P., et al. . (2023a). Associations between nausea and vomiting in pregnancy, disgust sensitivity, and first-trimester maternal serum free β-hCG and PAPP-A. Horm. Behav. 152:105360. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2023.105360, PMID: PubMed DOI

Kaňková Š., Takács L., Hlaváčová J., Calda P., Monk C., Havlíček J. (2023b). Disgust sensitivity in early pregnancy as a response to high pathogen risk. Front. Psychol. 14:1015927. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1015927 PubMed DOI PMC

Kaňková Š., Takács L., Krulová M., Hlaváčová J., Nouzová K., Hill M., et al. . (2022). Disgust sensitivity is negatively associated with immune system activity in early pregnancy: direct support for the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evol. Hum. Behav. 43, 234–241. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.02.001 DOI

Kayyal H., Yiannakas A., Chandran S. K., Khamaisy M., Sharma V., Rosenblum K. (2019). Activity of insula to basolateral amygdala projecting neurons is necessary and sufficient for taste valence representation. J. Neurosci. 39, 9369–9382. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0752-19.2019, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Kiefer S. W., Orr M. R. (1992). Taste avoidance, but not aversion, learning in rats lacking gustatory cortex. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 140–146. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.1.140 PubMed DOI

Knowles K. A., Jessup S. C., Olatunji B. O. (2018). Disgust in anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorders: recent findings and future directions. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 20, 68–10. doi: 10.1007/s11920-018-0936-5, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Knowles K. A., Viar-Paxton M. A., Riemann B. C., Jacobi D. M., Olatunji B. O. (2016). Is disgust proneness sensitive to treatment for OCD among youth?: examination of diagnostic specificity and symptom correlates. J. Anxiety Disord. 44, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.09.011, PMID: PubMed DOI

Koren T., Amer M., Krot M., Boshnak N., Ben-Shaanan T. L., Azulay-Debby H., et al. . (2021). Insular cortex neurons encode and retrieve specific immune responses. Cell 184, 5902–5915. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2021.10.013, PMID: PubMed DOI

Lavi K., Jacobson G. A., Rosenblum K., Lüthi A. (2018). Encoding of conditioned taste aversion in Cortico-amygdala circuits. Cell Rep. 24, 278–283. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.06.053, PMID: PubMed DOI

Lee K., An S. Y., Park J., Lee S., Kim H. F. (2023). Anatomical and functional comparison of the caudate tail in primates and the tail of the striatum in rodents: Implications for sensory information processing and habitual behavior. Molecules and Cells, 46, 461–469. doi: 10.14348/molcells.2023.0051, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Levin M. (2019). The computational boundary of a “self”: developmental bioelectricity drives multicellularity and scale-free cognition. Front. Psychol. 10:2688. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02688, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Levin M. (2022). Technological approach to mind everywhere: an experimentally-grounded framework for understanding diverse bodies and minds. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 16:768201. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.768201, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Levin M. (2023). “Collective intelligence of morphogenesis as a teleonomic process” in Evolution “on purpose”: teleonomy in living systems. eds. Corning P. A., Kauffman S. A., Noble D., Shapiro J. A., Vane-Wright R. I., Pross A. (Cambridge: MIT Press; ), 175–198.

Lutz H. (1949). Sur la production experimentale de la polyembryonie et de la monstruosite double chez les oiseaux. Archs Anat. Microsc. Morph. Exp. 38, 79–144.

Markose S. M. (2022). Complexification of eukaryote phenotype: adaptive immuno-cognitive systems as unique Gödelian blockchain distributed ledger. Biosystems 220:104718. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2022.104718, PMID: PubMed DOI

Maturana H. R., Varela F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. Dordrecht: Dreidel Pub. Co.

McEwen B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 840, 33–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x PubMed DOI

Mesulam M. M., Mufson E. J. (1982). Insula of the old world monkey. III: efferent cortical output and comments on function. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 38–52. doi: 10.1002/cne.902120104, PMID: PubMed DOI

Milkowska K., Galbarczyk A., Jasienska G. (2019). Disgust sensitivity in relation to menstrual cycle phase in women with and without an infection. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 31:e23233. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23233, PMID: PubMed DOI

Miłkowska K., Galbarczyk A., Klimek M., Zabłocka-Słowińska K., Jasienska G. (2021). Pathogen disgust, but not moral disgust, changes across the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 42, 402–408. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.03.002 DOI

Miller W. I. (1997). The anatomy of disgust. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Miyaura H., Iwata M. (2002). Direct and indirect inhibition of Th1 development by progesterone and glucocorticoids. J. Immunol. 168, 1087–1094. doi: 10.4049/jimmunol.168.3.1087 PubMed DOI

Moll J., de Oliveira-Souza R., Moll F. T., Ignácio F. A., Bramati I. E., Caparelli-Dáquer E. M., et al. . (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: a functional MRI study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 18, 68–78. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152236.46475.a7, PMID: PubMed DOI

Morgan J. T., Amaral D. G. (2013). Comparative analysis of the dendritic organization of principal neurons in the lateral and central nuclei of the rhesus macaque and rat amygdala. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 522, 689–176. doi: 10.1002/cne.23467, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Olatunji B. O., Armstrong T., Elwood L. (2017). Is disgust proneness associated with anxiety and related disorders? A qualitative review and meta-analysis of group comparison and correlational studies. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 613–648. doi: 10.1177/1745691616688879, PMID: PubMed DOI

Olatunji B. O., Cox R. C., Li I. (2020). Disgust regulation between menstrual cycle phases: differential effects of emotional suppression and reappraisal. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 68:101543. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.101543, PMID: PubMed DOI

Panksepp J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 407–422. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00012759 DOI

Panksepp J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Panksepp J. (2007). Criteria for basic emotions: is DISGUST a primary “emotion”? Cognit. Emot. 21, 1819–1828. doi: 10.1080/02699930701334302 DOI

Panksepp J., Biven L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: neural origins of human emotion. New York, NY: WW Norton and Company.

Parr T., Pezzulo G., Friston K. J. (2022). Active inference: the free energy principle in mind, brain, and behavior. London: MIT Press.

Phillips M. L., Senior C., Fahy T., David A. S. (1998). Disgust–the forgotten emotion of psychiatry. Br. J. Psychiatry 172, 373–375. doi: 10.1192/bjp.172.5.373, PMID: PubMed DOI

Polák J., Rádlová S., Janovcová M., Flegr J., Landová E., Frynta D. (2020). Scary and nasty beasts: self-reported fear and disgust of common phobic animals. Br. J. Psychol. 111, 297–321. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12409, PMID: PubMed DOI

Pujol J., Blanco-Hinojo L., Coronas R., Esteba-Castillo S., Rigla M., Martínez-Vilavella G., et al. . (2018). Mapping the sequence of brain events in response to disgusting food. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 369–380. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23848, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Rolls E. T. (1989). Information processing in the taste system of primates. J. Exp. Biol. 146, 141–164. doi: 10.1242/jeb.146.1.141 PubMed DOI

Rozin P., Fallon A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychol. Rev. 94, 23–41. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23 PubMed DOI

Sarolidou G., Axelsson J., Kimball B. A., Sundelin T., Regenbogen C., Lundström J. N., et al. . (2020). People expressing olfactory and visual cues of disease are less liked. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190272. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0272, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Sasson Y., Dekel S., Nacasch N., Chopra M., Zinger Y., Amital D., et al. . (2005). Posttraumatic obsessive–compulsive disorder: a case series. Psychiatry Res. 135, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.026, PMID: PubMed DOI

Schaller M., Duncan L. A. (2007). “The behavioral immune system: its evolution and social psychological implications” in Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognitions. eds. Forgas J. P., Haselton M. G., Von Hippel W. (London: Routledge; ), 293–307.

Schulkin J., Sterling P. (2019). Allostasis: a brain-centered, predictive mode of physiological regulation. Trends Neurosci. 42, 740–752. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.07.010, PMID: PubMed DOI

Schumacher A., Heinze K., Witte J., Poloski E., Linzke N., Woidacki K., et al. . (2013). Human chorionic gonadotropin as a central regulator of pregnancy immune tolerance. J. Immunol. 190, 2650–2658. doi: 10.4049/jimmunol.1202698 PubMed DOI

Siviy S. M., Panksepp J. (1985). Dorsomedial diencephalic involvement in the juvenile play of rats. Behav. Neurosci. 99, 1103–1113. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.99.6.1103, PMID: PubMed DOI

Solms M. (2018a). The scientific standing of psychoanalysis. BJPsych Int. 15, 5–8. doi: 10.1192/bji.2017.4, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Solms M. (2018b). The neurobiological underpinnings of psychoanalytic theory and therapy. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:294. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00294, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Solms M. (2021). Revision of drive theory. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 69, 1033–1091. doi: 10.1177/00030651211057041 PubMed DOI

Solms M., Turnbull O. (2018). The brain and the inner world: an introduction to the neuroscience of subjective experience. London: Routledge.

Sprengelmeyer R., Young A. W., Calder A. J., Karnat A., Lange H., Hömberg V., et al. . (1996). Loss of disgust. Brain 119, 1647–1665. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1647 PubMed DOI

Sprengelmeyer R., Young A. W., Mahn K., Schroeder U., Woitalla D., Büttner T., et al. . (2003). Facial expression recognition in people with medicated and unmedicated Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 41, 1047–1057. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00295-6, PMID: PubMed DOI

Sprengelmeyer R., Young A. W., Pundt I., Sprengelmeyer A., Calder A. J., Berrios G., et al. . (1997). Disgust implicated in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Proc. Royal Soc. Series B: Biol. Sci. 264, 1767–1773. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0245, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Steiner J. E., Glaser D., Hawilo M. E., Berridge K. C. (2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neuroscience and amp. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 53–74. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00051-8, PMID: PubMed DOI

Stern J., Shiramizu V. (2022). Hormones, ovulatory cycle phase and pathogen disgust: a longitudinal investigation of the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Horm. Behav. 138:105103. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105103, PMID: PubMed DOI

Stevenson R. J., Case T. I., Oaten M. J. (2009). Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 30, 363–368. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.02.005 DOI

Straube T., Weisbrod A., Schmidt S., Raschdorf C., Preul C., Mentzel H.-J., et al. . (2010). No impairment of recognition and experience of disgust in a patient with a right-hemispheric lesion of the insula and basal ganglia. Neuropsychologia 48, 1735–1741. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.022, PMID: PubMed DOI

Thorpe S. J., Patel S. P., Simonds L. M. (2003). The relationship between disgust sensitivity, anxiety and obsessions. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 1397–1409. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00058-5 PubMed DOI

Timmers A. D., Bossio J. A., Chivers M. L. (2018). Disgust, sexual cues, and the prophylaxis hypothesis. Evol. Psychol. Sci. 4:179, –190. doi: 10.1007/s40806-017-0127-3 DOI

Toronchuk J. A., Ellis G. F. (2007a). Disgust: sensory affect or primary emotional system? Cognit. Emot. 21, 1799–1818. doi: 10.1080/02699930701298515 DOI

Toronchuk J. A., Ellis G. F. (2007b). Criteria for basic emotions: seeking DISGUST? Cognit. Emot. 21, 1829–1832. doi: 10.1080/02699930701334419 DOI

Tuerke K. J., Limebeer C. L., Fletcher P. J., Parker L. A. (2012). Double dissociation between regulation of conditioned disgust and taste avoidance by serotonin availability at the 5-HT3Receptor in the posterior and anterior insular cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 13709–13717. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2042-12.2012, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Tybur J. M., Lieberman D., Griskevicius V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 103–122. Scopus. doi: 10.1037/a0015474 PubMed DOI

Valderrama J., Hansen S. K., Pato C., Phillips K., Knowles J., Pato M. T. (2020). Greater history of traumatic event exposure and PTSD associated with comorbid body dysmorphic disorder in a large OCD cohort. Psychiatry Res. 289:112962. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112962, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Van Overveld M., de Jong P. J., Peters M. L., van Hout W. J., Bouman T. K. (2008). An internet-based study on the relation between disgust sensitivity and emetophobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 22, 524–531. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.04.001, PMID: PubMed DOI

Varela F. G., Maturana H. R., Uribe R. (1974). Autopoiesis: the organization of living systems, its characterization and a model. Curr. Mod. Biol. 5, 187–196. doi: 10.1016/0303-2647(74)90031-8 PubMed DOI

Xie Z., Zhang X., Zhao M., Huo L., Huang M., Li D., et al. . (2022). The gut-to-brain axis for toxin-induced defensive responses. Cell 185, 4298–4316. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2022.10.001, PMID: PubMed DOI

Želaźniewicz A., Borkowska B., Nowak J., Pawłowski B. (2016). The progesterone level, leukocyte count and disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Physiol. Behav. 161, 60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.04.002, PMID: PubMed DOI

Żelaźniewicz A., Pawłowski B. (2015). Disgust in pregnancy and fetus sex—longitudinal study. Physiol. Behav. 139, 177–181. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.032, PMID: PubMed DOI

Zhong W., Shahbaz O., Teskey G., Beever A., Kachour N., Venketaraman V., et al. . (2021). Mechanisms of nausea and vomiting: current knowledge and recent advances in intracellular emetic signaling systems. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22:5797. doi: 10.3390/ijms22115797, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...