A standardized lexicon of body odor words crafted from 17 countries
Language English Country Great Britain, England Media electronic
Document type Journal Article, Dataset
PubMed
39987152
PubMed Central
PMC11846834
DOI
10.1038/s41597-025-04630-8
PII: 10.1038/s41597-025-04630-8
Knihovny.cz E-resources
- MeSH
- Smell * MeSH
- Olfactory Perception * MeSH
- Adult MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Nonverbal Communication MeSH
- Odorants * MeSH
- Check Tag
- Adult MeSH
- Humans MeSH
- Male MeSH
- Female MeSH
- Publication type
- Journal Article MeSH
- Dataset MeSH
Body odors offer a unique window into the physiological and psychological profile of the emitter. This information, broadcast in nonverbal communication, significantly shapes social interactions. However, effectively digitizing body odors requires a precise framework for perceptual operationalization. Previous research has used a very limited number of verbal terms, such as pleasant, intense, or attractive, which fails to adequately capture qualitative differences. To address this gap, we elicited body odor descriptions from 2,607 participants across 17 countries and 13 languages. All these descriptions are presented here in one dataset, together with a condensed list of 25 body odor words (BOW). Those terms reliably differentiated between body states, and were validated in a separate study with a different group of 155 perceivers. The dataset, available as a web application, provides a novel operationalization of body odor impressions, which is a precondition for studying olfaction in human nonverbal communication, for perception-based digitization of body odors and for comparative studies.
Centre for the Study of Labour and Human Factors University of Valparaíso Valparaíso Chile
Department of Clinical Neuroscience Karolinska Institutet Stockholm Sweden
Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford UK
Department of Psychology Hebrew University of Jerusalem Jerusalem Israel
Department of Psychology The Chinese University of Hong Kong Hong Kong Hong Kong
Faculty of Information Technology and Communication Sciences Tampere University Tampere Finland
German Center for Mental Health site Halle Jena Magdeburg Halle Jena Magdeburg Germany
Hotchkiss Brain Institute and Department of Psychiatry University of Calgary Calgary Canada
Institute for Materials Science Technische Universität Dresden Dresden Germany
Institute of Physical Chemistry Friedrich Schiller Universität Jena Jena Germany
Institute of Psychology University of Wroclaw Wroclaw Poland
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig Germany
School of Business Administration Hebrew University of Jerusalem Jerusalem Israel
School of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology Indore Indore India
Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry Western University London ON Canada
See more in PubMed
Schirmer, A., Croy, I., Liebal, K. & Schweinberger, S. Non-verbal effecting - animal research sheds light on human emotion communication. Biological Reviews, 100(1), 245-257, 10.1111/brv.13140 (2025). PubMed PMC
Loos, H. M. et al. Past, present, and future of human chemical communication research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 20, 20-44, 10.1177/17456916231188147 (2023). PubMed PMC
Olsson, M. J. et al. The scent of disease human body odor contains an early chemosensory cue of sickness. Psychological Science25, 817–823, 10.1177/0956797613515681 (2014). PubMed
Hoffmann, H. The aroma of arousal: Effects of menstrual cycle phase and women’s sexual arousal state on men’s responsiveness to women’s body odor. Biological Psychology142, 54–61, 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.01.012 (2019). PubMed
de Groot, J. H., Smeets, M. A., Kaldewaij, A., Duijndam, M. J. & Semin, G. R. Chemosignals communicate human emotions. Psychological science23, 1417–1424, 10.1177/0956797612445317 (2012). PubMed
Schäfer, L., Sorokowska, A., Sauter, J., Schmidt, A. H. & Croy, I. Body odours as a chemosignal in the mother-child relationship: new insights based on an human leucocyte antigen-genotyped family cohort. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B375, 10.1098/rstb.2019.0266 (2020). PubMed PMC
Drabińska, N. et al. A literature survey of all volatiles from healthy human breath and bodily fluids: the human volatilome. Journal of breath research15, 10.1088/1752-7163/abf1d0 (2021). PubMed
Owsienko, D. et al. Body odor samples from infants and post-pubertal children differ in their volatile profiles. Communications Chemistry7, 1–10, 10.1038/s42004-024-01131-4 (2024). PubMed PMC
Montuschi, P., Mores, N., Trové, A., Mondino, C. & Barnes, P. J. The electronic nose in respiratory medicine. Respiration85, 72–84, 10.1159/000340044 (2012). PubMed
Van De Goor, R. et al. Feasibility of electronic nose technology for discriminating between head and neck, bladder, and colon carcinomas. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology274, 1053–1060, 10.1007/s00405-016-4320-y (2017). PubMed PMC
Tozlu, B. H., Simsek, C., Aydemir, O. & Karavelioglu, Y. A high performance electronic nose system for the recognition of myocardial infarction and coronary artery diseases. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control64, 102247, 10.1016/j.bspc.2020.102247 (2021).
Sato, T. et al. Sniffer mice discriminate urine odours of patients with bladder cancer: A proof-of-principle study for non-invasive diagnosis of cancer-induced odours. Scientific reports7, 10.1038/s41598-017-15355-z (2017). PubMed PMC
Ko, B. A brief review of facial emotion recognition based on visual information. Sensors (Basel, Switzerland)18, 10.3390/s18020401 (2018). PubMed PMC
Brody, H. Medical imaging. Nature502, S81–S81, 10.1038/502S81a (2013). PubMed
Gildersleeve, K. A., Haselton, M. G., Larson, C. M. & Pillsworth, E. G. Body odor attractiveness as a cue of impending ovulation in women: Evidence from a study using hormone-confirmed ovulation. Hormones and Behavior61, 157–166, 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.11.005 (2012). PubMed
Lobmaier, J. S., Fischbacher, U., Wirthmuller, U. & Knoch, D. The scent of attractiveness: levels of reproductive hormones explain individual differences in women’s body odour. Proc Biol Sci285, 20181520, 10.1098/rspb.2018.1520 (2018). PubMed PMC
Nabergoj, D., Janeš, D., Fatur, K., Glavač, N. K. & Kreft, S. Influence of the human menstrual cycle on the perception of musks and substances responsible for body odour. Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology56, 565–576, 10.1134/s0022093020060095 (2020).
Keller, A. & Vosshall, L. B. Olfactory perception of chemically diverse molecules. BMC Neurosci17, 55, 10.1186/s12868-016-0287-2 (2016). PubMed PMC
Trimmer, C. et al. Genetic variation across the human olfactory receptor repertoire alters odor perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America116, 9475–9480, 10.1073/pnas.1804106115 (2019). PubMed PMC
Bierling, A. L., Croy, I., Hummel, T., Cuniberti, G. & Croy, A. Olfactory perception in relation to the physicochemical odor space. Brain Sciences11, 563, 10.3390/brainsci11050563 (2021). PubMed PMC
Singh, D. & Bronstad, P. M. Female body odour is a potential cue to ovulation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences268, 797–801, 10.1098/rspb.2001.1589 (2001). PubMed PMC
Havlíček, J., Dvořáková, R., Bartoš, L. & Flegr, J. Non-advertized does not mean concealed: body odour changes across the human menstrual cycle. Ethology112, 81–90, 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01125.x (2006).
van Nieuwenburg, D., de Groot, J. H. B. & Smeets, M. A. M. The subtle signaling strength of smells: A masked odor enhances interpersonal trust. Frontiers in Psychology10, 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01890 (2019). PubMed PMC
Majid, A. Human olfaction at the intersection of language, culture, and biology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences25, 111–123, 10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.005 (2021). PubMed
Allen, C., Havlícek, J., Williams, K. & Roberts, S. C. Perfume experts’ perceptions of body odors: Toward a new lexicon for body odor description. Journal of Sensory Studies33, e12314, 10.1111/joss.12314 (2018).
Croijmans, I. & Majid, A. Not all flavor expertise is equal: The language of wine and coffee experts. PLoS One11, 10.1371/journal.pone.0155845 (2016). PubMed PMC
Ferdenzi, C. et al. Variability of affective responses to odors: Culture, gender, and olfactory knowledge. Chemical Senses38, 175–186, 10.1093/chemse/bjs083 (2013). PubMed
Ferdenzi, C. et al. Individual differences in verbal and non-verbal affective responses to smells: Influence of odor label across cultures. Chemical Senses42, 37, 10.1093/chemse/bjw098 (2016). PubMed
Arshamian, A. et al. The perception of odor pleasantness is shared across cultures. Current Biology32, 2061–+, 10.1016/j.cub.2022.02.062 (2022). PubMed PMC
Sorokowski, P. et al. Is the perception of odour pleasantness shared across cultures and ecological conditions? evidence from amazonia, east africa, new guinea, malaysia and poland. Biology Letters20, 10.1098/rsbl.2024.0120 (2024). PubMed PMC
Huisman, J. L. A., van Hout, R. & Majid, A. Patterns of semantic variation differ across body parts: evidence from the japonic languages. Cognitive Linguistics32, 455–486, 10.1515/cog-2020-0079 (2021).
Lenochova, P., Roberts, S. C. & Havlicek, J. Methods of human body odor sampling: the effect of freezing. Chemical Senses34, 127–138, 10.1093/chemse/bjn067 (2009). PubMed
Löwe, B. P. & Spitzer, R. L.PHQ-D: Gesundheitsfragebogen Für Patienten (Pfizer GmbH, 2002).
Bierling, A. L. et al. BOW - a standardized lexicon of body odor words crafted from 17 countries, 10.17605/OSF.IO/RPZJK (2025). PubMed PMC
Majid, A., Burenhult, N., Stensmyr, M., de Valk, J. & Hansson, B. S. Olfactory language and abstraction across cultures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences373, 20170139, 10.1098/rstb.2017.0139 (2018). PubMed PMC