How are personality trait and profile agreement related?

. 2015 ; 6 () : 785. [epub] 20150609

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid26106356

It is argued that if we compute self-other agreement on some personality traits then we possess no or very little information about the individuals who are the targets of this judgment. This idea is largely based on two separate ways of computing self-other agreement: trait agreement (rT ) and profile agreement (rP ), which are typically associated with two different trait-centered and person-centered approaches in personality research. Personality traits of 4115 targets from Czech, Belgian, Estonian, and German samples were rated by themselves and knowledgeable informants. We demonstrate that trait agreement can be partialled into individual contributions so that it is possible to show how much each individual pair of judges contributes to agreement on a particular trait. Similarly, it is possible to decompose agreement between two personality profiles into the individual contributions of traits from which these profiles are assembled. If normativeness is separated from distinctiveness of personality scores and individual profiles are ipsatized, then mean profile agreement rP becomes identical to mean trait agreement r T . The views that trait-by-trait analysis does not provide information regarding accuracy level of a particular pair of judges and profile analysis does not permit assessment of the relative contributions of traits to overall accuracy are not supported.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Allik J., Realo A., McCrae R. R. (2013). Universality of the five-factor model of personality, in Personality Disorders and the Five Factor Model of Personality, eds Costa P. T., Widiger T. (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; ), 61–74.

Allik J., Realo A., Mõttus R., Borkenau P., Kuppens P., Hřebíčková M. (2010a). How people see others is different from how people see themselves: a replicable pattern across cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94, 168–182. 10.1037/a0020963 PubMed DOI

Allik J., Realo A., Mõttus R., Borkenau P., Kuppens P., Hřebíčková M. (2012). Person-fit to the five factor model of personality. Swiss J. Psychol. 71, 35–45. 10.1024/1421-0185/a000066 DOI

Allik J., Realo A., Mõttus R., Kuppens P. (2010b). Generalizability of self-other agreement from one personality trait to another. Pers. Individ. Dif. 48, 128–132. 10.1016/j.paid.2009.09.008 DOI

Asendorpf J. B. (1990). The measurement of individual consistency. Methodika 4, 1–23.

Asendorpf J. B. (1992). Beyond stability: predicting interindividual differences in intraindividual change. Eur. J. Pers. 6, 103–117. 10.1002/per.2410060204 DOI

Asendorpf J. B. (2000). Ideographic and nomothetic apporaches in psychology. Z. Psychol. 208, 72–90. 10.1026/0044-3409.208.12.72 PubMed DOI

Ashton M. C., Lee K. (2010). Trait and source factors in HEXACO-PI-R self- and observer reports. Eur. J. Pers. 24, 278–289. 10.1002/per.759 DOI

Ashton M. C., Lee K., Perugini M., Szarota P., de Vries R. E., Di Blas L., et al. . (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 356–366. 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.356 PubMed DOI

Bergman L. R., Trost K. (2006). The person-oriented versus the variable-oriented approach: are they complementary, opposites, or exploring different worlds? Merrill. Palmer. Q. J. Dev. Psychol. 52, 601–632. 10.1353/mpq.2006.0023 DOI

Bernieri F. J., Zuckerman M., Koestner R., Rosenthal R. (1994). Measuring person perception accuracy: another look at self-other agreement. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20, 367–378. 10.1177/0146167294204004 DOI

Berry J. B., Poortinga Y. H., Segall M. H., Dasen P. R. (2002). Cross-Cultural Psychology: Research and Applications, 2nd Edn Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Biesanz J. C., West S. G. (2000). Personality coherence: moderating self-other profile agreement and profile consensus. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 425–437. 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.425 PubMed DOI

Borkenau P., Hřebíčková M., Kuppens P., Realo A., Allik J. (2013a). Sex differences in variability in personality: a study in four samples. J. Pers. 81, 49–60. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00784.x PubMed DOI

Borkenau P., McCrae R. R., Terracciano A. (2013b). Do men vary more than women in personality? A study in 51 cultures. J. Res. Pers. 47, 135–144. 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.12.001 PubMed DOI PMC

Borkenau P., Zaltauskas K. (2009). Effects of Self-enhancement on agreement on personality profiles. Eur. J. Pers. 23, 107–123. 10.1002/per.707 DOI

Borsboom D. (2005). Measuring the Mind: Conceptual Issues in Contemporary Psychometrics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Borsboom D., Mellenbergh G. J., van Heerden J. (2003). The theoretical status of latent variables. Psychol. Rev. 110, 203–219. 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.203 PubMed DOI

Bruner J. S., Tagiuri R. (1954). The perception of people, in Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 2, ed Lindscy G. (Cambridge: Addison-Wesley; ), 634–654.

Connelly B. S., Ones D. S. (2010). An other perspective on personality: meta-analytic integration of observers' accuracy and predictive validity. Psychol. Bull. 136, 1092–1122. 10.1037/a0021212 PubMed DOI

Connolly J. J., Kavanagh E. J., Viswesvaran C. (2007). The convergent validity between self and observer ratings of personality: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 15, 110–117. 10.1111/j.1468-2389.2007.00371.x DOI

Cronbach L. J. (1955). Process affecting scores on “understanding others” and “assumed similarity.” Psychol. Bull. 52, 177–193. 10.1037/h0044919 PubMed DOI

Cronbach L. J., Gleser G. (1953). Assessing similarity between profiles. Psychol. Bull. 50, 456–473. 10.1037/h0057173 PubMed DOI

De Fruyt F., De Bolle M., McCrae R. R., Terracciano A., Costa P. T. (2009). Assessing the universal structure of personality in early adolescence the NEO-PI-R and NEO-PI-3 in 24 cultures. Assessment 16, 301–311. 10.1177/1073191109333760 PubMed DOI PMC

De Los Reyes A., Thomas S. A., Goodman K. L., Kundey S. M. A. (2013). Principles underlying the use of multiple informants' reports. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 123–149. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185617 PubMed DOI PMC

Dobewall H., Realo A., Allik J., Esko T., Metspalu A. (2013). Self-other agreement in happiness and life-satisfaction: the role of personality traits. Soc. Indic. Res. 114, 479–492. 10.1007/s11205-012-0157-y DOI

Epstein S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1097–1126. 10.1037/0022-3514.37.7.1097 DOI

Epstein S. (1980). The stability of behavior: II. Implications for psychological research. Am. Psychol. 35, 790–806. 10.1037/0003-066X.35.9.790 DOI

Funder D. C. (1997). The Personality Puzzle. New York, NY: Norton.

Funder D. C. (1999). Personality Judgement: A Realistic Approach to Person Perception. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Funder D. C., Colvin C. R. (1997). Congruence of others' and self-judgements on personality, in Handbook of Personality Psychology, eds Hogan R., Johnson J., Briggs S. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 617–647.

Furr R. M. (2008). A framework for profile similarity: integrating similarity, normativeness, and distinctiveness. J. Pers. 76, 1267–1316. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00521.x PubMed DOI

Furr R. M. (2009). Profile analysis in person-situation integration. J. Res. Pers. 43, 196–207. 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.08.002 DOI

Furr R. M. (2010). The double-entry intraclass correlation as an index of profile similarity: meaning, limitations, and alternatives. J. Pers. Assess. 92, 1–15. 10.1080/00223890903379134 PubMed DOI

Gage N. L., Cronbach L. J. (1955). Conceptual and methodological problems in interpersonal perception. Psychol. Rev. 62, 411–422. 10.1037/h0047205 PubMed DOI

Goldberg L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am. Psychol. 48, 26–34. 10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26 PubMed DOI

Henrich J., Heine S. J., Norenzayan A. (2010a). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466, 29–29. 10.1038/466029a PubMed DOI

Henrich J., Heine S. J., Norenzayan A. (2010b). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 1–75. 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X PubMed DOI

Hoekstra H. A., Ormel J., DeFruyt F. (1996). NEO Persoonlijkheidsvragenlisten NEO-PI-R en NEO-FF-I. Handleigin. Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger.

Hřebíčková M. (2002). Internal consistency of the Czech version of the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R). Cesk. Psychol. 46, 521–535.

Jones D. (2010). A WEIRD view of human nature skews psychologists' studies. Science 328, 1627–1627. 10.1126/science.328.5986.1627 PubMed DOI

Kandler C., Riemann R., Spinath F. M., Angleitner A. (2010). Sources of variance in personality facets: a multiple-rater twin study of self-peer, peer-peer, and self-self (dis)agreement. J. Pers. 78, 1565–1594. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00661.x PubMed DOI

Kenny D. A., West T. V. (2010). Similarity and agreement in self-and other perception: a meta-analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 14, 196–213. 10.1177/1088868309353414 PubMed DOI

Kenny D. A., West T. V., Malloy T. E., Albright L. (2006). Componential analysis of interpersonal perception data. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 282–294. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_1 PubMed DOI

Kenny D. A., Winquist L. A. (2001). The measurement of interpersonal sensitivity: consideration of design, components, and unit of analysis, in Interpersonal Sensitivity: Theory and Measurement, eds Hall J. A., Bernieri F. J. (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Erlbaum; ), 265–302.

Kolar D. W., Funder D. C., Colvin C. R. (1996). Comparing the accuracy of personality judgments by the self and knowledgeable others. J. Pers. 64, 311–337. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00513.x PubMed DOI

Lee K., Ashton M. C. (2008). The HEXACO personality factors in the indigenous personality lexicons of english and 11 other languages. J. Pers. 76, 1001–1053. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00512.x PubMed DOI

Leitsalu L., Haller T., Esko T., Tammesoo M.-L., Alavere H., Snieder H., et al. . (2014). Cohort Profile: Estonian Biobank of the Estonian Genome Center, University of Tartu. Int. J. Epidemiol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1093/ije/dyt268 PubMed DOI

Magnusson D., Torestäd B. (1993). A holistic view of personality: a model revisited. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 44, 427–452. 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.002235 DOI

McCrae R. R. (1993). Agreement of personality profiles across observers. Multivariate Behav. Res. 28, 25–40. 10.1207/s15327906mbr2801_2 PubMed DOI

McCrae R. R. (1994). The counterpoint of personality assessment: self-reports and observer ratings. Assessment 1, 159–172. 10.1177/1073191194001002006 PubMed DOI

McCrae R. R. (2008). A note on some measures of profile agreement. J. Pers. Assess. 90, 105–109. 10.1080/00223890701845104 PubMed DOI PMC

McCrae R. R., Allik J. (2002). The Five-Factor Model of Personality Across Cultures. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic; Plenum Publishers.

McCrae R. R., Costa P. T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 81–90. 10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81 PubMed DOI

McCrae R. R., Costa P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am. Psychol. 52, 509–516. 10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509 PubMed DOI

McCrae R. R., Costa P. T., Jr. (2010). NEO Inventories Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

McCrae R. R., Costa P. T., Martin T. A. (2005). The NEO-PI-3: a more readable revised neo personality inventory. J. Pers. Assess. 84, 261–270. 10.1207/s15327752jpa8403_05 PubMed DOI

McCrae R. R., Costa P. T., Martin T. A., Oryol V. E., Rukavishnikov A. A., Senin I. G., et al. (2004). Consensual validation of personality traits across cultures. J. Res. Pers. 38, 179–201. 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00056-4 DOI

McCrae R. R., Jang K. L., Livesley W. J., Riemann R., Angleitner A. (2001). Sources of structure: genetic, environmental, and artifactual influences on the covariation of personality traits. J. Pers. 69, 511–535. 10.1111/1467-6494.694154 PubMed DOI

Molenaar P. C. M., Campbell C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 112–117. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x DOI

Ostendorf F., Angleitner A. (2004). NEO-PI-R: NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar Nach Costa und McCrae [NEO Personality Inventory According to Costa and McCrae]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Pelham B. W. (1993). The idiographic nature of human personality: examples of the idiographic self-concept. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 665–677. 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.665 PubMed DOI

Taft R. (1955). The ability to judge people. Psychol. Bull. 52, 1–23. 10.1037/h0044999 PubMed DOI

Vazire S. (2006). Informant reports: a cheap, fast, and easy method for personality assessment. J. Res. Pers. 40, 472–481. 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.03.003 DOI

Watson D., Hubbard B., Wiese D. (2000). Self-other agreement in personality and affectivity: the role of acquaintanceship, trait visibility, and assumed similarity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 546–558. 10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.546 PubMed DOI

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

Unusual Configurations of Personality Traits Indicate Multiple Patterns of Their Coalescence

. 2018 ; 9 () : 187. [epub] 20180220

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...