Effect of fragrance use on discrimination of individual body odor
Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články
PubMed
26300812
PubMed Central
PMC4528100
DOI
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01115
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- Klíčová slova
- body odor, deodorant, identification, olfaction, smell, triangle test,
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
Previous research suggests that artificial fragrances may be chosen to complement or enhance an individual's body odor, rather than simply masking it, and that this may create an odor blend with an emergent quality that is perceptually distinguishable from body odor or fragrance alone. From this, it can be predicted that a new emergent odor might be more easily identified than an individual's body odor in isolation. We used a triangle test paradigm to assess whether fragrance affects people's ability to distinguish between individual odors. Six male and six female donors provided axillary odor samples in three conditions (without fragrance, wearing their own fragrance, and wearing an assigned fragrance). In total, 296 female and 131 male participants selected the odd one from three odor samples (two from one donor, one from another; both of the same sex). We found that participants could discriminate between the odors at above chance levels in all three odor conditions. Olfactory identification ability (measured using Sniffin' Sticks) positively predicted discrimination performance, and sex differences in performance were also observed, with female raters being correct more often than men. Success rates were also higher for odors of male donors. Additionally, while performance was above chance in all conditions, individual odor discrimination varied across the three conditions. Discrimination rate was significantly higher in the "no fragrance" condition than either of the fragranced conditions. Importantly, however, discrimination rate was also significantly higher in the "own fragrance" condition than the "assigned fragrance" condition, suggesting that naturally occurring variance in body odor is more preserved when blended with fragrances that people choose for themselves, compared with other fragrances. Our data are consistent with the idea that fragrance choices are influenced by fragrance interactions with an individual's own body odor.
Department of Zoology Charles University Prague Czech Republic
Division of Psychology School of Natural Sciences University of Stirling Stirling UK
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Brand G., Millot J. L. (2001). Sex differences in human olfaction: between evidence and enigma. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B 54, 259–270. 10.1080/02724990143000045 PubMed DOI
Cardesín A., Alobid I., Benítez P., Sierra E., Haro J. D., Bernal-Sprekelsen M., et al. (2006). Barcelona Smell Test—24 (BAST-24): validation and smell characteristics in the healthy Spanish population. Rhinology 24, 83–89. PubMed
Cernoch J. M., Porter R. H. (1985). Recognition of maternal axillary odors by infants. Child Dev. 56, 1593–1598. 10.2307/1130478 PubMed DOI
Chen D., Haviland-Jones J. (2000). Human olfactory communication of emotion. Percept. Mot. Skills 91, 771–781. 10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.771 PubMed DOI
Doty R. L. (1981). Olfactory communication in humans. Chem. Senses 6, 351–376. 10.1093/chemse/6.4.351 DOI
Doty R. L., Cameron E. L. (2010). Sex differences and reproductive hormone influences on human odor perception. Physiol. Behav. 97, 213–228. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.032 PubMed DOI PMC
Doty R. L., Orndorff M. M., Leyden J. J., Kligman A. (1978). Communication of gender from human axillary odors: relationship to perceived intensity and hedonicity. Behav. Biol. 23, 373–380. 10.1016/S0091-6773(78)91393-7 PubMed DOI
Ferdenzi C., Schaal B., Roberts S. C. (2010). Family scents: developmental changes in the perception of kin body odor? J. Chem. Ecol. 36, 847–854. 10.1007/s10886-010-9827-x PubMed DOI
Fialová J., Havlíček J. (2012). Perception of emotion-related body odours in humans. Anthropologie 50, 95–110.
Havlíček J., Dvorakova R., Bartos L., Flegr J. (2006). Non-advertized does not mean concealed: body odour changes across the human menstrual cycle. Ethology 112, 81–90. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01125.x DOI
Havlíček J., Roberts S. C. (2009). MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 497–512. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007 PubMed DOI
Havlíček J., Roberts S. C. (2013). “The perfume-body odour complex: an insightful model for culture–gene coevolution?,” in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, Vol. 12, eds East M. L., Dehnhard M. (New York: Springer; ), 185–195.
Hummel T., Bensafi M., Nikolaus J., Knecht M., Laing D. G., Schaal B. (2007a). Olfactory function in children assessed with psychophysical and electrophysiological techniques. Behav. Brain Res. 180, 133–138. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.040 PubMed DOI
Hummel T., Kobal G., Gudziol H., Mackay-Sim A. (2007b). Normative data for the “Sniffin’ Sticks” including tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds: an upgrade based on a group of more than 3,000 subjects. Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 264, 237–243. 10.1007/s00405-006-0173-0 PubMed DOI
Kohoutová D., Rubešová A., Havlíček J. (2011). Shaving of axillary hair has only a transient effect on perceived body odor pleasantness. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 569–581. 10.1007/s00265-011-1305-0 DOI
Kuukasjärvi S., Eriksson C. J. P., Koskela E., Mappers T., Nissinen K., Rantala M. J. (2004). Attractiveness of women’s body odors over the menstrual cycle: the role of oral contraceptives and receiver sex. Behav. Ecol. 15, 579–584. 10.1093/beheco/arh050 DOI
Lenochová P., Roberts S. C., Havlíček J. (2009). Methods of human body odor sampling: the effect of freezing. Chem. Senses 34, 127–138. 10.1093/chemse/bjn067 PubMed DOI
Lenochová P., Vohnoutová P., Roberts S. C., Oberzaucher E., Grammer K., Havlíček J. (2012). Psychology of fragrance use: perception of individual odor and perfume blends reveals a mechanism for idiosyncratic effects on fragrance choice. PLoS ONE 7:e33810. 10.1371/journal.pone.0033810 PubMed DOI PMC
Lord T., Kasprzak M. (1989). Identification of self through olfaction. Percept. Mot. Skills 69, 219–224. 10.2466/pms.1989.69.1.219 PubMed DOI
Milinski M., Wedekind C. (2001). Evidence for MHC-correlated perfume preferences in humans. Behav. Ecol. 12, 140–149. 10.1093/beheco/12.2.140 DOI
Moshkin M., Litvinova N., Litvinova E. A., Bedareva A., Lutsyuk A., Gerlinskaya L. (2012). Scent recognition of infected status in humans. J. Sex. Med. 9, 3211–3218. 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02562.x PubMed DOI
Penn D. J., Oberzaucher E., Grammer K., Fischer G., Soini H. A., Wiesler D., et al. (2007). Individual and gender fingerprints in human body odour. J. R. Soc. Interface 4, 331–340. 10.1098/rsif.2006.0182 PubMed DOI PMC
Platek S. M., Burch R. L., Gallup G. G. (2001). Sex differences in olfactory self-recognition. Physiol. Behav. 73, 635–640. 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00539-X PubMed DOI
Porter R. H., Balogh R. D., Cernoch J. M., Franchi C. (1986). Recognition of kin through characteristic body odors. Chem. Senses 11, 389–395. 10.1093/chemse/11.3.389 DOI
Porter R. H., Cernoch J. M., McLaughlin F. J. (1983). Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiol. Behav. 30, 151–154. 10.1016/0031-9384(83)90051-3 PubMed DOI
Raimbault C., Saliba E., Porter R. H. (2007). The effect of the odour of mother’s milk on breastfeeding behaviour of premature neonates. Acta Paediatr. 96, 368–371. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00114.x PubMed DOI
Rikowski A., Grammer K. (1999). Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 266, 869–874. 10.1098/rspb.1999.0717 PubMed DOI PMC
Roberts S. C., Gosling L. M., Carter V., Petrie M. (2008). MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives. Proc. R. B Biol. Sci. 275, 2715–2722. 10.1098/rspb.2008.0825 PubMed DOI PMC
Roberts S. C., Gosling L. M., Spector T. D., Miller P., Penn D. J., Petrie M. (2005). Body odor similarity in noncohabiting twins. Chem. Senses 30, 651–656. 10.1093/chemse/bji058 PubMed DOI
Roberts S. C., Havlíček J. (2012). “Evolutionary psychology and fragrance design,” in Applied Evolutionary Psychology, ed. Craig Roberts S. (New York: Oxford University Press; ), 330–348.
Rodriguez S., Steer C. D., Farrow A., Golding J., Day I. N. M. (2013). Dependence of deodorant usage on ABCC11 genotype: scope for personalized genetics in personal hygiene. J. Investig. Dermatol. 133, 1760–1767. 10.1038/jid.2012.480 PubMed DOI PMC
Schleidt M. (1980). Personal odor and nonverbal communication. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 225–231. 10.1016/0162-3095(80)90009-6 DOI
Schleidt M., Hold B., Attili G. (1981). A cross-cultural study on the attitude towards personal odors. J. Chem. Ecol. 7, 19–31. 10.1007/BF00988632 PubMed DOI
Singh D., Bronstad P. M. (2001). Female body odour is a potential cue to ovulation. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 268, 797–801. 10.1098/rspb.2001.1589 PubMed DOI PMC
Thornhill R., Gangestad S. W. (1999). The scent of symmetry: a human sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 175–201. 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00005-7 DOI
Wedekind C., Seebeck T., Bettens F., Paepke A. J. (1995). MHC—dependent mate preferences in humans. Proc. Biol. Sci. 260, 245–249. 10.1098/rspb.1995.0087 PubMed DOI
Emotional expression in human odour
Human olfactory communication: current challenges and future prospects
Body Odor Based Personality Judgments: The Effect of Fragranced Cosmetics