No evidence for self-recognition in a small passerine, the great tit (Parus major) judged from the mark/mirror test
Jazyk angličtina Země Německo Médium print-electronic
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články
PubMed
28762195
PubMed Central
PMC5640729
DOI
10.1007/s10071-017-1121-7
PII: 10.1007/s10071-017-1121-7
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- Klíčová slova
- Colour mark, Great tit, Mirror test, Parus major, Self-recognition,
- MeSH
- chování zvířat * MeSH
- kognice * MeSH
- Passeriformes fyziologie MeSH
- rozpoznávání (psychologie) * MeSH
- zraková percepce MeSH
- zvířata MeSH
- Check Tag
- mužské pohlaví MeSH
- ženské pohlaví MeSH
- zvířata MeSH
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
Self-recognition is a trait presumed to be associated with high levels of cognition and something previously considered to be exclusive to humans and possibly apes. The most common test of self-recognition is the mark/mirror test of whether an animal can understand that it sees its own reflection in a mirror. The usual design is that an animal is marked with a colour spot somewhere on the body where the spot can only be seen by the animal by using a mirror. Very few species have passed this test, and among birds, only magpies have been affirmatively demonstrated to pass it. In this study, we tested great tits (Parus major), small passerines, that are known for their innovative foraging skills and good problem-solving abilities, in the mirror self-recognition test. We found no indication that they have any ability of this kind and believe that they are unlikely to be capable of this type of self-recognition.
Department of Biology Lund University Lund Sweden
Department of Zoology Charles University Prague Czech Republic
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Amsterdam B. Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Dev Psychobiol. 1972;5:297–305. doi: 10.1002/dev.420050403. PubMed DOI
Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Cole EF, Cockburn A, Thornton A, Sheldon BC. Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature. 2015;518:538–541. doi: 10.1038/nature13998. PubMed DOI PMC
Branch, Kozlowski, Pravosudov Elevation related variation in aggressive response to mirror image in mountain chickadees. Behaviour. 2015;152:667–676. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003248. DOI
Brodin A, Urhan AU. Interspecific observational memory in a non-caching Parus species, the great tit Parus major. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2014;68:649–656. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1679-2. DOI
Brodin A, Urhan AU. Sex differences in learning ability in a common songbird, the great tit. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2015;69:237–241. doi: 10.1007/s00265-014-1836-2. DOI
Broesch T, Callaghan T, Henrich J, Murphy C, Rochat P. Cultural variations in children’s mirror self-recognition. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2011;42:1018–1029. doi: 10.1177/0022022110381114. DOI
Cauchard L, Boogert NJ, Lefebvre L, Dubois F, Doligez B. Problem-solving performance is correlated with reproductive success in a wild bird population. Anim Behav. 2013;85:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.005. DOI
Chang L, Fang Q, Zhang S, Poo M, Gong N. Mirror-induced self-directed behaviors in rhesus monkeys after visual-somatosensory training. Curr Biol. 2015;25:212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.016. PubMed DOI
Chensky EJ, Ficken MS. Responses of black-capped chickadees to mirrors. Wilson Bull. 1982;94:590–594.
Clary D, Kelly DM. Graded mirror self-recognition by Clark’s nutcrackers. Sci Rep UK. 2016;6:36459. doi: 10.1038/srep36459. PubMed DOI PMC
Cramp S, Perrins CM (eds) (1993) Parus major Great tit. In: Handbook of the birds of Europe the Middle East and North Africa: the birds of the Western Palearctic. vol 7. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 255–279
de Waal FBM. The thief in the mirror. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:1621–1622. PubMed PMC
Derégnaucourt S, Bovet D. The perception of self in birds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;69:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.039. PubMed DOI
Dunbar RIM, Schultz S. Evolution in the social brain. Science. 2007;317:1344–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.1145463. PubMed DOI
Epstein R, Lanza RP, Skinner BF. ”Self-awareness” in the pigeon. Science. 1981;212:695–696. doi: 10.1126/science.212.4495.695. PubMed DOI
Gallup GGJ. Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science. 1970;167:86–87. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3914.86. PubMed DOI
Gallup GGJ. Self-awareness and the evolution of social intelligence. Behav Process. 1998;42:239–247. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(97)00079-X. PubMed DOI
Hampton RE. Introductory biological statistics. Dubuque, Melbourne, Oxford: WCB Publishers; 1994.
Heyes C. Reflections on self-recognition in primates. Anim Behav. 1994;47:909–919. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1123. DOI
Lefebvre L, Boogert NJ. Avian social learning. In: Breed MD, Moore J, editors. Encyclopedia of animal behavior. Oxford: Academic Press, Elsevier; 2010. pp. 124–130.
Olkowicz S, Kocourek M, Lučan RK, Porteš M, Fitch WT, Herculano-Houzel S, Němec P. Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. PNAS. 2016;113(26):7255–7260. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517131113. PubMed DOI PMC
Pepperberg IM, Garcia SE, Jackson EC, Marconi S. Mirror use by African Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) J Comp Psychol. 1995;109:182–195. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.2.182. DOI
Plotnik JM, de Waal FBM, Reiss D. Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:17053–17057. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608062103. PubMed DOI PMC
Povinelli DJ. Failure to find self-recognition in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in contrast to their use of mirror cues to discover hidden food. J Comp Psychol. 1989;103:122–131. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.103.2.122. DOI
Povinelli DJ, Rulf AB, Landau KR, Bierschwale DT. Self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): distribution, ontogeny, and patterns of emergence. J Comp Psychol. 1993;107:347–372. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.107.4.347. PubMed DOI
Povinelli DJ, Gallup GGJ, Eddy TJ, Bierschwale DT, Engstrom MC, Perriloux HK, Toxopeus IB. Chimpanzees recognize themselves in mirrors. Anim Behav. 1997;53:1083–1088. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0303. DOI
Prior H, Schwarz A, Güntürkün O. Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:1642–1650. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202. PubMed DOI PMC
Reader SM, Laland KN. Social intelligence, innovation and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:4436–4441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062041299. PubMed DOI PMC
Reiss D, Marino L. Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:5937–5942. doi: 10.1073/pnas.101086398. PubMed DOI PMC
Sasvári L. Observational learning in great, blue and marsh tits. Anim Behav. 1979;27:767–771. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90012-5. DOI
Shettleworth SJ. Cognition, evolution and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
Soler M, Pérez-Contreras T, Peralta-Sánchez JM. Mirror–mark tests performed on jackdaws reveal potential methodological problems in the use of stickers in avian mark-test studies. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:1–10. PubMed PMC
Suddendorf T, Butler DL. The nature of visual self-recognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:121–127. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.004. PubMed DOI
Uchino E, Watanabe S. Self-recognition in pigeons revisited. J Exp Anal Behav. 2014;102:327–334. doi: 10.1002/jeab.112. PubMed DOI
Ünver E, Garland A, Tabrik S, Güntürkün O. Sneaking a peek: pigeons use peripheral vision (not mirrors) to find hidden food. Anim Cogn. 2017;20(4):677–688. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1090-x. PubMed DOI