• This record comes from PubMed

Behavioral and neurophysiological evidence for increased cognitive flexibility in late childhood

. 2016 Jun 28 ; 6 () : 28954. [epub] 20160628

Language English Country Great Britain, England Media electronic

Document type Journal Article, Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't

Executive functions, like the capacity to control and organize thoughts and behavior, develop from childhood to young adulthood. Although task switching and working memory processes are known to undergo strong developmental changes from childhood to adulthood, it is currently unknown how task switching processes are modulated between childhood and adulthood given that working memory processes are central to task switching. The aim of the current study is therefore to examine this question using a combined cue- and memory-based task switching paradigm in children (N = 25) and young adults (N = 25) in combination with neurophysiological (EEG) methods. We obtained an unexpected paradoxical effect suggesting that memory-based task switching is better in late childhood than in young adulthood. No group differences were observed in cue-based task switching. The neurophysiological data suggest that this effect is not due to altered attentional selection (P1, N1) or processes related to the updating, organization, and implementation of the new task-set (P3). Instead, alterations were found in the resolution of task-set conflict and the selection of an appropriate response (N2) when a task has to be switched. Our observation contrasts findings showing that cognitive control mechanisms reach their optimal functioning in early adulthood.

See more in PubMed

Norman D. A. & Shallice T. Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. Technical Report No. 8006 (ERIC Clearinghouse, Washington, DC, 1980).

Miyake A. et al.. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex ‘Frontal Lobe’ Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognit. Psychol. 41, 49–100 (2000). PubMed

Allport D. A., Styles E. A. & Hsieh S. Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. Attention and performance XV, 421–452 (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1994).

Rogers R. D. & Monsell S. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 124, 207–231 (1995).

Braver T. S., Reynolds J. R. & Donaldson D. I. Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching. Neuron 39, 713–726 (2003). PubMed

Dove A., Pollmann S., Schubert T., Wiggins C. J. & Yves von Cramon D. Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 103–109 (2000). PubMed

Gohil K., Dippel G. & Beste C. Questioning the role of the frontopolar cortex in multi-component behavior - a TMS/EEG study. Sci. Rep. 6, 22317 (2016). PubMed PMC

Hikosaka O. & Isoda M. Switching from automatic to controlled behavior: cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 154–161 (2010). PubMed PMC

Beste C. et al.. Mechanisms mediating parallel action monitoring in fronto-striatal circuits. NeuroImage 62, 137–146 (2012). PubMed

Yildiz A. & Beste C. Parallel and serial processing in dual-tasking differentially involves mechanisms in the striatum and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 3131–3142 (2015). PubMed

Zhang R., Stock A.-K., Fischer R. & Beste C. The system neurophysiological basis of backward inhibition. Brain Struct. Funct., 1–13, 10.1007/s00429-016-1186-0 (2016). PubMed DOI

Diamond A. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry In Principles of frontal lobe (eds Stuss DT & Knight RT) 466–503 (Oxford University Press, London, 2002).

Davidson M. C., Amso D., Anderson L. C. & Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44, 2037–2078 (2006). PubMed PMC

Ellefson M. R., Shapiro L. R. & Chater N. Asymmetrical switch costs in children. Cogn. Dev. 21, 108–130 (2006).

Crone E. A., Ridderinkhof K. R., Worm M., Somsen R. J. M. & van der Molen M. W. Switching between spatial stimulus-response mappings: a developmental study of cognitive flexibility. Dev. Sci. 7, 443–455 (2004). PubMed

Huizinga M., Dolan C. V. & van der Molen M. W. Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2017–2036 (2006). PubMed

Murphy J. W., Foxe J. J. & Molholm S. Neuro-oscillatory mechanisms of intersensory selective attention and task switching in school-aged children, adolescents and young adults. Dev. Sci. 19, 469–487, 10.1111/desc.12316 (2015). PubMed DOI PMC

Welsh M. C., Pennington B. F. & Groisser D. B. A normative‐developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 7, 131–149 (1991).

Rubia K. et al.. Progressive increase of frontostriatal brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event-related tasks of cognitive control. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 973–993 (2006). PubMed PMC

Gajewski P. D., Hengstler J. G., Golka K., Falkenstein M. & Beste C. The Met-allele of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism enhances task switching in elderly. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 2327.e7–19 (2011). PubMed

Darki F. & Klingberg T. The Role of Fronto-Parietal and Fronto-Striatal Networks in the Development of Working Memory: A Longitudinal Study. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1587–1595 (2015). PubMed

Swanson H. L. Individual and age-related differences in children’s working memory. Mem. Cognit. 24, 70–82 (1996). PubMed

Luciana M. & Nelson C. A. The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children. Neuropsychologia 36, 273–293 (1998). PubMed

Gajewski P. D. & Falkenstein M. Training-Induced Improvement of Response Selection and Error Detection in Aging Assessed by Task Switching: Effects of Cognitive, Physical, and Relaxation Training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6(130), 1–18 (2012). PubMed PMC

Gajewski P. D., Kleinsorge T. & Falkenstein M. Electrophysiological correlates of residual switch costs. Cortex 46, 1138–1148 (2010). PubMed

Barceló F. An information theoretical approach to task-switching: evidence from cognitive brain potentials in humans. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 1(13), 10.3389/neuro.09.013.2007 (2008). PubMed DOI PMC

Gajewski P. D. et al.. Effects of aging and job demands on cognitive flexibility assessed by task switching. Biol. Psychol. 85, 187–199 (2010). PubMed

Gehring W. J., Bryck R. L., Jonides J., Albin R. L. & Badre D. The mind’s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting. Psychophysiology 40, 572–585 (2003). PubMed

Karayanidis F., Coltheart M., Michie P. T. & Murphy K. Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory and poststimulus components of task switching. Psychophysiology 40, 329–348 (2003). PubMed

Lamm C., Zelazo P. D. & Lewis M. D. Neural correlates of cognitive control in childhood and adolescence: disentangling the contributions of age and executive function. Neuropsychologia 44, 2139–2148 (2006). PubMed

Davis E. P., Bruce J., Snyder K. & Nelson C. A. The X-Trials: Neural Correlates of an Inhibitory Control Task in Children and Adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 432–443 (2003). PubMed

Jonkman L. M., Lansbergen M. & Stauder J. E. A. Developmental differences in behavioral and event-related brain responses associated with response preparation and inhibition in a go/nogo task. Psychophysiology 40, 752–761 (2003). PubMed

Herrmann C. S. & Knight R. T. Mechanisms of human attention: event-related potentials and oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 465–476 (2001). PubMed

Wascher E., Schneider D., Hoffmann S., Beste C. & Sänger J. When compensation fails: attentional deficits in healthy ageing caused by visual distraction. Neuropsychologia 50, 3185–3192 (2012). PubMed

Beste C., Stock A.-K., Ness V., Hoffmann R. & Saft C. Evidence for divergent effects of neurodegeneration in Huntington’s disease on attentional selection and neural plasticity: implications for excitotoxicity. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 1437–1447 (2015). PubMed

Schneider D., Beste C. & Wascher E. On the time course of bottom-up and top-down processes in selective viusal attention: an EEG study. Psychophysiology 49, 1492–1503, 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01462.x2012 (2012). PubMed DOI

Kray J. Task-set switching under cue-based versus memory-based switching conditions in younger and older adults. Brain Res. 1105, 83–92 (2006). PubMed

Koch I. The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 488–492 (2003). PubMed

Astle D. E. & Scerif G. Interactions between attention and visual short-term memory (VSTM): What can be learnt from individual and developmental differences? Neuropsychologia 49, 1435–1445 (2011). PubMed

Weeda W. D., van der Molen M. W., Barceló F. & Huizinga M. A diffusion model analysis of developmental changes in children’s task switching. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 126, 178–197 (2014). PubMed

Dibbets P. & Jolles J. The Switch Task for Children: Measuring mental flexibility in young children. Cogn. Dev. 21, 60–71 (2006).

Kray J., Eber J. & Lindenberger U. Age differences in executive functioning across the lifespan: The role of verbalization in task preparation. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 115, 143–165 (2004). PubMed

Luck S. J., Heinze H. J., Mangun G. R. & Hillyard S. A. Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 528–542 (1990). PubMed

Wascher E. & Beste C. Tuning Perceptual Competition. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1057–1065 (2010). PubMed

Sharma A., Kraus N. J., McGee T. & Nicol T. G. Developmental changes in P1 and N1 central auditory responses elicited by consonant-vowel syllables. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Potentials Sect. 104, 540–545 (1997). PubMed

Coch D. & Meade G. N1 and P2 to words and wordlike stimuli in late elementary school children and adults. Psychophysiology 53, 115–128, 10.1111/psyp.12567 (2016) PubMed DOI PMC

Luck S. J. et al.. Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 887–904 (1994). PubMed

Barceló F., Muñoz-Céspedes J. M., Pozo M. A. & Rubia F. J. Attentional set shifting modulates the target P3b response in the Wisconsin card sorting test. Neuropsychologia 38, 1342–1355 (2000). PubMed

Beste C., Ness V., Falkenstein M. & Saft C. On the role of fronto-striatal neural synchronization processes for response inhibition--evidence from ERP phase-synchronization analyses in pre-manifest Huntington’s disease gene mutation carriers. Neuropsychologia 49, 3484–3493 (2011). PubMed

Nicholson R., Karayanidis F., Poboka D., Heathcote A. & Michie P. T. Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. Psychophysiology 42, 540–554 (2005). PubMed

Getzmann S., Hanenberg C., Lewald J., Falkenstein M. & Wascher E. Effects of age on electrophysiological correlates of speech processing in a dynamic ‘cocktail-party’ situation. Front. Neurosci. 9, 341 (2015). PubMed PMC

Karayanidis F., Whitson L. R., Heathcote A. & Michie P. T. Variability in proactive and reactive cognitive control processes across the adult lifespan. Front. Psychol. 2, 318 (2011). PubMed PMC

Gajewski P. D., Stoerig P. & Falkenstein M. ERP—Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Res. 1189, 127–134 (2008). PubMed

Wu S. et al.. The neural dynamic mechanisms of asymmetric switch costs in a combined Stroop-task-switching paradigm. Sci. Rep. 5, 10240 (2015). PubMed PMC

Folstein J. R. & Van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology 45(1), 152–170 (2007). PubMed PMC

Swanson H. L. What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Dev. Psychol. 35, 986–1000 (1999). PubMed

Selemon L. D. A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e238 (2013). PubMed PMC

Giedd J. N. et al.. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863 (1999). PubMed

Müller V. I., Langner R., Cieslik E. C., Rottschy C. & Eickhoff S. B. Interindividual differences in cognitive flexibility: influence of gray matter volume, functional connectivity and trait impulsivity. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 2401–2414 (2015). PubMed PMC

Mückschel M., Stock A.-K. & Beste C. Psychophysiological Mechanisms of Interindividual Differences in Goal Activation Modes During Action Cascading. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2120–2129 (2014). PubMed

Nunez P. L. & Pilgreen K. L. The Spline-Laplacian in Clinical Neurophysiology: A Method to Improve EEG Spatial Resolution. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 8, 397–413 (1991). PubMed

Find record

Citation metrics

Loading data ...

Archiving options

Loading data ...