Attention Bias and Recognition of Sexual Images

. 2020 ; 11 () : 556071. [epub] 20201102

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid33224052

Attention to sexual stimuli is necessary for the development of sexual response, yet while there is some evidence of attention bias in favor of sexual stimuli, the direction and magnitude of the effect remain unknown. A high-powered sample of 113 participants was tested using the dot-probe task (DPT) and picture recognition task (PRT) to measure visuospatial attention to erotic images. Participants showed no attention bias in the DPT (r B = 0.201, p = 0.064) but were significantly better at recognizing erotic rather than neutral or training pictures (d = 1.445 and 1.461, respectively, both p < 0.001). This indicates that spatial attention bias to sexual pictures is small, negligible, possibly even non-existent, or else the DPT is not a reliable tool to assess it. Results of the PRT, on the other hand, show that sexual stimuli are prioritized in memory and this should be explored in future research.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Bailey J. M., Gaulin S., Agyei Y., Gladue B. A. (1994). Effects of gender and sexual orientation on evolutionarily relevant aspects of human mating psychology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66 1081–1093. 10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1081 PubMed DOI

Barlow D. H. (1986). Causes of sexual dysfunction. The Role of Anxiety and Cognitive Interference. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 54 140–148. 10.1037/0022-006X.54.2.140 PubMed DOI

Beard C., Amir N. (2010). Attention bias for sexual words in female sexual dysfunction. J. Sex Marit. Ther. 36 216–226. 10.1080/00926231003719616 PubMed DOI PMC

Beck A. T., Steer R. A., Brown G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Bradley M. M., Karlsson M., Lang P. J. (2017). Assessing hedonic bias in emotional scene memory: implications for clinical research. Z. Psychol. 225 223–231. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000306 DOI

Brauer M., Van Leeuwen M., Janssen E., Newhouse S. K., Heiman J. R., Laan E. (2012). Attentional and affective processing of sexual stimuli in women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41 891–905. 10.1007/s10508-011-9820-7 PubMed DOI

Bush S. I., Geer J. H. (2001). Implicit and explicit memory of neutral, negative emotional, and sexual information. Arch. Sex. Behav. 30 615–631. 10.1023/A:1011915001416 PubMed DOI

Conaglen H. M., Evans I. M. (2006). Pictorial cues and sexual desire: an experimental approach. Arch. Sex. Behav. 35 197–212. 10.1007/s10508-005-9000-8 PubMed DOI

Cooper R. M., Langton S. R. H. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behav. Res. Ther. 44 1321–1329. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.004 PubMed DOI

Cummins R. G., Gong Z. H., Reichert T. (2020). The impact of visual sexual appeals on attention allocation within advertisements: an eye-tracking study. Int. J. Advert. 39, 1–25. 10.1080/02650487.2020.1772656 DOI

Dagnino B., Navajas J., Sigman M. (2012). Eye Fixations Indicate Men’s Preference for Female Breasts or Buttocks. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41 929–937. 10.1007/s10508-012-9945-3 PubMed DOI

Dawson S. J., Chivers M. L. (2016). Gender-specificity of initial and controlled visual attention to sexual stimuli in androphilic women and gynephilic men. PLoS One 11:e0152785. 10.1371/journal.pone.0152785 PubMed DOI PMC

de Jong D. C. (2009). The role of attention in sexual arousal: implications for treatment of sexual dysfunction. J. Sex Res. 46 237–248. 10.1080/00224490902747230 PubMed DOI

Dewitte M. (2016). Gender differences in implicit processing of sexual stimuli. Eur. J. Pers. 30 107–124. 10.1002/per.2031 DOI

Doornwaard S. M., van Den Eijnden R. J. J. M., Johnson A., ter Bogt T. F. M. (2014). exposure to sexualized media content and selective attention for sexual cues: an experimental study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 41 357–364. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.037 DOI

Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., Buchner A. (2007). G∗Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39 175–191. PubMed

Frohlich P., Meston C. (2002). Sexual functioning and self-reported depressive symptoms among college women. J. Sex Res. 39 321–325. 10.1080/00224490209552156 PubMed DOI

Fromberger P., Jordan K., Steinkrauss H., Von Herder J., Stolpmann G., Kröner-Herwig B., et al. (2013). Eye Movements in Pedophiles: automatic and Controlled Attentional Processes while Viewing Prepubescent Stimuli. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122 587–599. 10.1037/a0030659 PubMed DOI

Fromberger P., Jordan K., von Herder J., Steinkrauss H., Nemetschek R., Stolpmann G., et al. (2012). Initial orienting towards sexually relevant stimuli: preliminary evidence from eye movement measures. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41 919–928. 10.1007/s10508-011-9816-3 PubMed DOI PMC

Ganesan A., Morandini J. S., Veldre A., Hsu K. J., Dar-Nimrod I. (2020). Ethnic differences in visual attention to sexual stimuli among asian and white heterosexual women and men. Pers. Individ. Differ. 155:109630 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109630 DOI

Geer J. H., Bellard H. S. (1996). Sexual content induced delays in unprimed lexical decisions: gender and context effects. Arch. Sex. Behav. 25 379–395. 10.1007/BF02437581 PubMed DOI

Geer J. H., McGlone M. S. (1990). Sex differences in memory for erotica. Cogn. Emot. 4 71–78. 10.1080/02699939008406766 DOI

Gross J. J., John O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85 348–362. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 PubMed DOI

Hennink-Kaminski H. J., Reichert T. (2011). Using sexual appeals in advertising to sell cosmetic surgery: a content analysis from 1986 to 2007. Sex. Cult. 15 41–55. 10.1007/s12119-010-9081-y DOI

Imhoff R., Barker P., Schmidt A. F. (2019). To What Extent Do Erotic Images Elicit Visuospatial versus Cognitive Attentional Processes? Consistent Support for a (Non-Spatial) Sexual Content-Induced Delay. Arch. Sex. Behav. 49 531–550. 10.1007/s10508-019-01512-0 PubMed DOI

Ioannidis J. P. A., Munafò M. R., Fusar-Poli P., Nosek B. A., David S. P. (2008). Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences: detection, prevalence and prevention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18 235–241. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010 PubMed DOI PMC

JASP Team (2019). JASP (Version 0.11). Amsterdam: JASP.

Jiang M. Y. W., Vartanian L. R. (2018). A review of existing measures of attentional biases in body image and eating disorders research. Aust. J. Psychol. 70 3–17. 10.1111/ajpy.12161 DOI

Kagerer S., Wehrum S., Klucken T., Walter B., Vaitl D., Stark R. (2014). Sex attracts: investigating individual differences in attentional bias to sexual stimuli. PLoS One 9:e107795. 10.1371/journal.pone.0107795 PubMed DOI PMC

Kruijt A.-W., Parsons S., Fox E. (2019). A meta-analysis of bias at baseline in RCTs of attention bias modification: no evidence for dot-probe bias towards threat in clinical anxiety and PTSD. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128 563–573. 10.1037/abn0000406 PubMed DOI

Lang P. J., Bradley M. M., Cuthbert B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. Available online at: http://www.unifesp.br/dpsicobio/adap/instructions.pdf (accessed March 16, 2019).

Loftus G. R. (1972). Eye fixations and recognition memory for pictures. Cogn. Psychol. 3 525–551. 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90021-7 DOI

Lykins A. D., Meana M., Kambe G. (2006). Detection of differential viewing patterns to erotic and non-erotic stimuli using eye-tracking methodology. Arch. Sex. Behav. 35 569–575. 10.1007/s10508-006-9065-z PubMed DOI

MacLeod C., Mathews A., Tata P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95 15–20. 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15 PubMed DOI

Marchewka A., Zurawski L., Jednoróg K., Grabowska A. (2014). The Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS): introduction to a Novel, Standardized, Wide-Range, High-Quality, Realistic Picture Database. Behav. Res. Methods 46 596–610. 10.3758/s13428-013-0379-1 PubMed DOI PMC

Martini P., Maljkovic V. (2009). Short-term memory for pictures seen once or twice. Vision Res. 49 1657–1667. 10.1016/j.visres.2009.04.007 PubMed DOI PMC

Mechelmans D. J., Irvine M., Banca P., Porter L., Mitchell S., Mole T. B., et al. (2014). Enhanced attentional bias towards sexually explicit cues in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours. PLoS One 9:e105476. 10.1371/journal.pone.0105476 PubMed DOI PMC

Miller M. A., Fillmore M. T. (2010). The effect of image complexity on attentional bias toward alcohol-related images in adult drinkers. Addiction 105 883–890. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02860.x PubMed DOI PMC

Mogg K., Philippot P., Bradley B. P. (2004). Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 113 160–165. 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.160 PubMed DOI

Morandini J. S., Spence B., Dar-Nimrod I., Lykins A. D. (2020). Do bisexuals have a bisexual viewing pattern? Arch. Sex. Behav. 49 489–502. 10.1007/s10508-019-01514-y PubMed DOI

Navon D., Margalit B. (1983). Allocation of attention according to informativeness in visual recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 35 497–512. 10.1080/14640748308402484 PubMed DOI

Oosterwijk S., van der Leij A. R., Rotteveel M. (2017). Associating LIPS and SWOLLEN: delayed attentional disengagement following words in sex contexts. Cogn. Emot. 31 1197–1210. 10.1080/02699931.2016.1208150 PubMed DOI

Penke L., Asendorpf J. B. (2008). Beyond Global Sociosexual orientations: a more differentiated look at sociosexuality and its effects on courtship and romantic relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95 1113–1135. 10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1113 PubMed DOI

Posner M. I., Snyder C. R., Davidson B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 109 160–174. 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160 PubMed DOI

Pottage C. L., Schaefer A. (2012). Visual attention and emotional memory: recall of aversive pictures is partially mediated by concurrent task performance. Emotion 12 33–38. 10.1037/a0024574 PubMed DOI

Prause N., Janssen E., Hetrick W. P. (2008). Attention and emotional responses to sexual stimuli and their relationship to sexual desire. Arch. Sex. Behav. 37 934–949. 10.1007/s10508-007-9236-6 PubMed DOI

Ptáček R., Raboch J., Vňuková M., Hlinka J., Anders M. (2016). Beckova Škála Deprese BDI-II - Standardizace a Využití v Praxi. Česk. Slov. Psychiatr. 112 270–274.

R Core Team (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Reichert T., Alvaro E. (2001). The effects of sexual information on ad and brand processing and recall. Southwest. Mass Commun. J. 17 9–17.

Reichert T., Carpenter C. (2003). An update on sex in magazine advertising: 1983 to 2003. J. Mass Commun. Q. 81 823–837. 10.1177/107769900408100407 DOI

Reichert T., Lambiase J., Morgan S., Carstarphen M., Zavoina S. (1999). Cheesecake and beefcake: no matter how you slice it, sexual explicitness in advertising continues to increase. J. Mass Commun. Q. 76 7–20. 10.1177/107769909907600102 DOI

Rupp H. A., Wallen K. (2007). Sex differences in viewing sexual stimuli: an eye-tracking study in men and women. Horm. Behav. 51 524–533. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.008 PubMed DOI

Schmukle S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the Dot Probe Task. Eur. J. Pers. 19 595–605. 10.1002/per.554 DOI

Schneider W., Eschman A., Zuccolotto A. (2002). E–Prime (Version 2.0). Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

Seehuus M. (2015). Discrepant Attentional Biases Toward Sexual Stimuli. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont.

Snowden R. J., Curl C., Jobbins K., Lavington C., Gray N. S. (2016). Automatic direction of spatial attention to male versus female stimuli: a comparison of heterosexual men and women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 45 843–853. 10.1007/s10508-015-0678-y PubMed DOI PMC

Spiering M., Everaerd W. (2007). “The Sexual Unconscious,” in The Psychophysiology of Sex, ed. Janssen E. (Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press; ), 166–184.

Stanislaw H., Todorov N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 31 137–149. 10.3758/BF03207704 PubMed DOI

Staugaard S. R. (2009). Reliability of two versions of the dot-probe task using photographic faces. Psychol. Sci. Q. 51 339–350.

Strahler J., Baranowski A. M., Walter B., Huebner N., Stark R. (2019). Attentional bias toward and distractibility by sexual cues: a meta-analytic integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 105 276–287. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.015 PubMed DOI

Wirtz J. G., Sparks J. V., Zimbres T. M. (2017). The effect of exposure to sexual appeals in advertisements on memory, attitude, and purchase intention: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Advert. 37 168–198. 10.1080/02650487.2017.1334996 DOI

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...