Warmth and competence perceptions of key protagonists are associated with containment measures during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence from 35 countries
Jazyk angličtina Země Anglie, Velká Británie Médium electronic
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články, práce podpořená grantem
PubMed
36481750
PubMed Central
PMC9732048
DOI
10.1038/s41598-022-25228-9
PII: 10.1038/s41598-022-25228-9
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- MeSH
- COVID-19 * epidemiologie prevence a kontrola MeSH
- lidé MeSH
- pandemie prevence a kontrola MeSH
- průřezové studie MeSH
- Check Tag
- lidé MeSH
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
- práce podpořená grantem MeSH
It is crucial to understand why people comply with measures to contain viruses and their effects during pandemics. We provide evidence from 35 countries (Ntotal = 12,553) from 6 continents during the COVID-19 pandemic (between 2021 and 2022) obtained via cross-sectional surveys that the social perception of key protagonists on two basic dimensions-warmth and competence-plays a crucial role in shaping pandemic-related behaviors. Firstly, when asked in an open question format, heads of state, physicians, and protest movements were universally identified as key protagonists across countries. Secondly, multiple-group confirmatory factor analyses revealed that warmth and competence perceptions of these and other protagonists differed significantly within and between countries. Thirdly, internal meta-analyses showed that warmth and competence perceptions of heads of state, physicians, and protest movements were associated with support and opposition intentions, containment and prevention behaviors, as well as vaccination uptake. Our results have important implications for designing effective interventions to motivate desirable health outcomes and coping with future health crises and other global challenges.
Astana Medical University Astana Kazakhstan
Comenius University in Bratislava Bratislava Slovakia
Copenhagen Business School Frederiksberg Denmark
Department of Psychology Durham University South Road Durham DH1 3LE UK
Eastern Mediterranean University Famagusta Cyprus
Esade Ramon Llull University Barcelona Spain
FernUniversität in Hagen Hagen Germany
GESIS Leibniz Institut für Sozialwissenschaften Mannheim Germany
Ilia State University Tbilisi Georgia
Jagiellonian University Krakow Poland
M Narikbayev KAZGUU University Astana Kazakhstan
National University of Uzbekistan Tashkent Uzbekistan
Rutgers University New Brunswick USA
Sofia University St Kliment Ohridski Sofia Bulgaria
Swinburne University of Technology Melbourne Australia
SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities Warsaw Poland
Tampere University Tampere Finland
Taras Shevchenko National University of Kyiv Kyiv Ukraine
The Czech Academy of Sciences Prague Czechia
The University of British Columbia Vancouver Canada
Transilvania University of Brasov Brașov Romania
Universidad Católica de Cuyo National Scientific and Technical Research Council San Juan Argentina
Universidade Portucalense Porto Portugal
Université Catholique de Louvain Louvain la Neuve Belgium
University of Bergen Bergen Norway
University of Bern Bern Switzerland
University of Brasilia Brasília Brazil
University of Canterbury Christchurch New Zealand
University of Cyprus Nicosia Cyprus
University of Ghana Accra Ghana
University of Innovative and Social Economics Tashkent Uzbekistan
University of Limerick Limerick Ireland
University of Milano Bicocca Milan Italy
University of Porto Porto Portugal
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Davies S. Pandemics and the consequences of COVID-19. Econ. Aff. 2020;40:131–137. doi: 10.1111/ecaf.12415. DOI
Mathieu E, et al. A global database of COVID-19 vaccinations. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021;5:947–953. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01122-8. PubMed DOI
Douglas KM. COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2021;24:270–275. doi: 10.1177/1368430220982068. DOI
Søraa RA, et al. Othering and deprioritizing older adults’ lives: Ageist discourses during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. J. Psychol. 2020;16:532–541. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v16i4.4127. PubMed DOI PMC
Khan ML, Malik A, Ruhi U, Al-Busaidi A. Conflicting attitudes: Analyzing social media data to understand the early discourse on COVID-19 passports. Technol. Soc. 2022;68:101830. doi: 10.1016/j.techsoc.2021.101830. PubMed DOI PMC
Pascual-Ferrá P, Alperstein N, Barnett DJ, Rimal RN. Toxicity and verbal aggression on social media: Polarized discourse on wearing face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic. Big Data Soc. 2021;8:205395172110235. doi: 10.1177/20539517211023533. DOI
Burke PF, Masters D, Massey G. Enablers and barriers to COVID-19 vaccine uptake: An international study of perceptions and intentions. Vaccine. 2021;39:5116–5128. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2021.07.056. PubMed DOI PMC
Dong G, et al. Self-interest bias in the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-cultural comparison between the United States and China. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2021;52:663–679. doi: 10.1177/00220221211025739. DOI
Enea V, et al. Intentions to be vaccinated against COVID-19: The role of prosociality and conspiracy beliefs across 20 countries. Health Commun. 2022;0:1–10. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2021.2018179. PubMed DOI
Pagliaro S, et al. Trust predicts COVID-19 prescribed and discretionary behavioral intentions in 23 countries. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0248334. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248334. PubMed DOI PMC
Romano A, et al. Cooperation and trust across societies during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2021;52:622–642. doi: 10.1177/0022022120988913. DOI
Schumpe BM, et al. Predictors of adherence to public health behaviors for fighting COVID-19 derived from longitudinal data. Sci. Rep. 2022;12:3824. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-04703-9. PubMed DOI PMC
Van Bavel JJ, et al. National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:517. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-27668-9. PubMed DOI PMC
Zhu N, Smetana JG, Chang L. Acceptance of Society-level and individual-level preventive measures during the COVID-19 pandemic among college students in three societies. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2021;52:606–621. doi: 10.1177/0022022121995971. DOI
Visintin EP. Contact with older people, ageism, and containment behaviours during the COVID -19 pandemic. J. Commu. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2021;31:314–325. doi: 10.1002/casp.2504. PubMed DOI PMC
Alston L, Meleady R, Seger CR. Can past intergroup contact shape support for policies in a pandemic? Processes predicting endorsement of discriminatory Chinese restrictions during the COVID-19 crisis. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2022;25:122–132. doi: 10.1177/1368430220959710. DOI
Ahluwalia SC, Edelen MO, Qureshi N, Etchegaray JM. Trust in experts, not trust in national leadership, leads to greater uptake of recommended actions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Risk Hazards Crisis Public Policy. 2021;12:283–302. doi: 10.1002/rhc3.12219. PubMed DOI PMC
Siegrist M, Earle TC, Gutscher H. Test of a trust and confidence model in the applied context of electromagnetic field (EMF) risks. Risk Anal. 2003;23:705–716. doi: 10.1111/1539-6924.00349. PubMed DOI
Wynen J, et al. Taking a COVID-19 vaccine or not? Do trust in government and trust in experts help us to understand vaccination intention? Adm. Soc. 2022 doi: 10.1177/00953997211073459. DOI
Fiske ST, Cuddy AJC, Glick P, Xu J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002;82:878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878. PubMed DOI
Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Glick P. The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007;92:631–648. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631. PubMed DOI
Abele AE, Ellemers N, Fiske ST, Koch A, Yzerbyt V. Navigating the social world: Toward an integrated framework for evaluating self, individuals, and groups. Psychol. Rev. 2021;128:290–314. doi: 10.1037/rev0000262. PubMed DOI
Froehlich L, Schulte I. Warmth and competence stereotypes about immigrant groups in Germany. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0223103. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223103. PubMed DOI PMC
Kotzur PF, Friehs M-T, Asbrock F, van Zalk MHW. Stereotype content of refugee subgroups in Germany. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2019;49:1344–1358. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2585. DOI
Friehs MT, Aparicio-Lukassowitz F, Wagner U. US tereotype content of occupational groups in Germany. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2022;52:459–475. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12872. DOI
He JC, Kang SK, Tse K, Toh SM. Stereotypes at work: Occupational stereotypes predict race and gender segregation in the workforce. J. Vocat. Behav. 2019;115:103318. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2019.103318. DOI
Kervyn N, Chan E, Malone C, Korpusik A, Ybarra O. Not all disasters are equal in the public’s eye: The negativity effect on warmth in brand perception. Soc. Cogn. 2014;32:256–275. doi: 10.1521/soco.2014.32.3.256. DOI
Fiske ST, Nicolas G, Bai X. The stereotype content model: How we make sense of individuals and groups. In: Van Lange PAM, Higgins ET, Kruglanski AW, editors. Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. 3. The Guilford Press; 2021. pp. 392–410.
Sevillano V, Fiske ST. Stereotypes, emotions, and behaviors associated with animals: A causal test of the stereotype content model and BIAS map. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2019;22:879–900. doi: 10.1177/1368430219851560. DOI
Bick, N., Froehlich, L., Friehs, M.-T., Kotzur, P. F., & Landmann, H. Social evaluation at a distance - Facets of stereotype content about student groups in higher distance education. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol.35, 12 (2022).
Tajfel H, Turner J. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Hatch MJ, Schultz M, editors. Organizational Identity: A Reader. Oxford University Press; 1979. pp. 56–65.
Koch A, et al. Groups’ warmth is a personal matter: Understanding consensus on stereotype dimensions reconciles adversarial models of social evaluation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2020;89:103995. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103995. DOI
Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Prentice-Hall, Inc; 1986.
Zaccaro SJ, Blair V, Peterson C, Zazanis M. Collective efficacy. In: Maddux JE, editor. Self-efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment. Springer; 1995. pp. 305–330.
Lee K, Worsnop CZ, Grépin KA, Kamradt-Scott A. Global coordination on cross-border travel and trade measures crucial to COVID-19 response. Lancet. 2020;395:1593–1595. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)31032-1. PubMed DOI PMC
Miskulin M, et al. Vaccination attitudes and experiences of medical doctors in Croatia amid the COVID-19 pandemic: A social roles conflict? Vaccines. 2022;10:399. doi: 10.3390/vaccines10030399. PubMed DOI PMC
Young M, Marotta R, Lee I, Phan J, Jacobs RJ. Differences in attitudes of front-line clinicians, healthcare workers, and non-healthcare workers toward COVID-19 safety protocols. Cureus. 2022;14:e20936. PubMed PMC
Ullah I, Khan KS, Tahir MJ, Ahmed A, Harapan H. Myths and conspiracy theories on vaccines and COVID-19: Potential effect on global vaccine refusals. Vacunas. 2021;22:93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.vacun.2021.01.001. PubMed DOI PMC
Ajzen I, Fishbein M, Lohmann S, Albarracín D. The influence of attitudes on behavior. In: Albarracín D, Johnson BT, Zanna MP, editors. The Handbook of Attitudes. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2005. pp. 173–221.
Ajzen I, Fishbein M. The prediction of behavior from attitudinal and normative variables. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1970;6:466–487. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(70)90057-0. DOI
Ajzen I, Fishbein M. Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychol. Bull. 1977;84:888–918. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888. DOI
Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broad-based behavioral science. Behav. Brain Sci. 2010;33:111–135. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000725. DOI
Brunswik E. Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychol. Rev. 1955;62:193–217. doi: 10.1037/h0047470. PubMed DOI
Brunswik E. Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments. University of California Press; 1956.
Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Multiple-group factor analysis alignment. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2014;21:495–508. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.919210. DOI
Lasco G. Medical populism and the COVID-19 pandemic. Glob. Public Health. 2020;15:1417–1429. doi: 10.1080/17441692.2020.1807581. PubMed DOI
Wagner U, Friehs M-T, Kotzur PF. Das Bild der Polizei bei jungen Studierenden [The image of the police in the eyes of German students] Poliz. Wiss. 2020;3:16–23.
Southwell PL. The effect of political alienation on voter turnout, 1964–2000. J. Polit. Mil. Sociol. 2008;36:131–145.
Stoker G, Evans M. The, “democracy-politics paradox”: The dynamics of political alienation. Democr. Theory. 2014;1:26–36. doi: 10.3167/dt.2014.010203. DOI
Bartusevičius H, Bor A, Jørgensen F, Petersen MB. The psychological burden of the COVID-19 pandemic is associated with antisystemic attitudes and political violence. Psychol. Sci. 2021;32:1391–1403. doi: 10.1177/09567976211031847. PubMed DOI
Timoshenkova EP. Angela Merkel’s leadership lessons: The secret of political longevity (2013–2021) Her. Russ. Acad. Sci. 2022;92:S119–S125. doi: 10.1134/S1019331622080111. DOI
Day G, Robert G, Leedham-Green K, Rafferty AM. An outbreak of appreciation: A discursive analysis of tweets of gratitude expressed to the National Health Service at the outset of the COVID-19 pandemic. Health Expect. 2022;25:149–162. doi: 10.1111/hex.13359. PubMed DOI PMC
Garcia LP. Gratidão ao sistema único de saúde do Brasil. Epidemiol. E Serviços Saúde. 2020;29:e2020333. doi: 10.1590/s1679-49742020000500024. PubMed DOI
Rahaman MdS, Rahman MdM, Ali Reza SM, Reza MN, Chowdhury MdS. Thank you, COVID-19: Positive social psychology towards the new normal. J. Public Aff. 2021 doi: 10.1002/pa.2766. PubMed DOI PMC
Yun EK, Kim JO, Byun HM, Lee GG. Topic modeling and keyword network analysis of news articles related to nurses before and after. J. Korean Acad. Nurs. 2021;51:442–453. doi: 10.4040/jkan.20287. PubMed DOI
Festinger L. Informal social communication. Psychol. Rev. 1950;57:271–282. doi: 10.1037/h0056932. PubMed DOI
Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat. 1954;7:117–140. doi: 10.1177/001872675400700202. DOI
Cornwell JFM, Bajger AT, Higgins ET. Judging political hearts and minds: How political dynamics drive social judgments. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2015;41:1053–1068. doi: 10.1177/0146167215589720. PubMed DOI
Friehs M-T, et al. Examining the structural validity of stereotype content scales: A preregistered re-analysis of published data and discussion of possible future directions. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2022;35:1–18. doi: 10.5334/irsp.613. DOI
Bunge EM, et al. The changing epidemiology of human monkeypox: A potential threat? A systematic review. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 2022;16:e0010141. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0010141. PubMed DOI PMC
Raju E, Boyd E, Otto F. Stop blaming the climate for disasters. Commun. Earth Environ. 2022;3:1–2. doi: 10.1038/s43247-021-00332-2. DOI
Lichterman A. The peace movement and the Ukraine war: Where to now? J. Peace Nucl. Disarm. 2022;5:185–197. doi: 10.1080/25751654.2022.2060634. DOI
Fiske ST, North MS. Measures of stereotyping and prejudice. In: Boyle GJ, Saklofiske DH, Matthews G, editors. Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. Academic Press; 2015. pp. 684–718.
Harkness J. Questionnaire translation. In: Harkness J, Van De Vijver FJR, Mohler PP, editors. Cross-Cultural Survey Methods. Wiley; 2003. pp. 35–36.
Kotzur PF, et al. ‘Society thinks they are cold and/or incompetent, but I do not’: Stereotype content ratings depend on instructions and the social group’s location in the stereotype content space. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2020;59:1018–1042. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12375. PubMed DOI
GESIS Panel Team GESIS panel special survey on the coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 outbreak in Germany, ZA5667 Datenfile Version 1.1.0. GESIS Datenarchiv. 2020 doi: 10.4232/1.13520. DOI
Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2001;8:430–457. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5. DOI
Brislin RW. Comparative research methodology: Cross-cultural studies. Int. J. Psychol. 1976;11:215–229. doi: 10.1080/00207597608247359. DOI
Goh JX, Hall JA, Rosenthal R. Mini meta-analysis of your own studies: Some arguments on why and a primer on how. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass. 2016;10:535–549. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12267. DOI
Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2. Erlbaum; 1988.