Infants' reliance on rhythm to distinguish languages: A critical review
Jazyk angličtina Země Spojené státy americké Médium print-electronic
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články, přehledy
Grantová podpora
21-09797S
Grantová Agentura České Republiky
24-11139M
Grantová Agentura České Republiky
PubMed
39215603
DOI
10.1111/infa.12613
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- MeSH
- jazyk (prostředek komunikace) MeSH
- kojenec MeSH
- lidé MeSH
- percepce řeči fyziologie MeSH
- periodicita MeSH
- podněty * MeSH
- vývoj řeči * MeSH
- Check Tag
- kojenec MeSH
- lidé MeSH
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
- přehledy MeSH
This article reviews empirical methods and findings on early language discrimination, questioning rhythm-class based hypotheses on language discrimination in infancy, as well as the assumption that early language discrimination is driven primarily (or solely) by temporal prosodic cues. The present work argues that within-rhythm class discrimination which - according to the rhythmic hypothesis - is not applicable very early in life, has not been sufficiently tested with infants under 4 months of age, that familiarity with a language is not a prerequisite for its discrimination from another rhythmically similar language, and that the temporal rhythm properties may not universally be the primary cues to language discrimination. Although rhythm taxonomy is now by many understood as outdated, some developmental literature still draws on the assumption that rhythm classification determines infants' language discrimination; other studies consider rhythm along a continuous scale and only a few account for cues to language discrimination other than temporal ones. It is proposed that studies on early language discrimination systematically test the contribution of other than temporal rhythm cues, similarly to recent work on multidimensional psychoacoustic salience in the acquisition of segmental categories.
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Abboub, N., Nazzi, T., & Gervain, J. (2016). Prosodic grouping at birth. Brain and Language, 162, 46–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.002
Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Edinburgh University Press.
Arenillas‐Alcón, S., Costa‐Faidella, J., Ribas‐Prats, T., Gómez‐Roig, M. D., & Escera, C. (2021). Neural encoding of voice pitch and formant structure at birth as revealed by frequency‐following responses. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 6660. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598‐021‐85799‐x
Arvaniti, A. (2009). Rhythm, timing and the timing of rhythm. Phonetica, 66(1–2), 46–63. https://doi.org/10.1159/000208930
Arvaniti, A. (2012). The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 40(3), 351–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2012.02.003
Barry, W., Andreeva, B., & Koreman, J. (2009). Do rhythm measures reflect perceived rhythm? Phonetica, 66(1–2), 78–94. https://doi.org/10.1159/000208932
Barry, W. J., Andreeva, B., Russo, M., Dimitrova, S., & Kostadinova, T. (2003). Do rhythm measures tell us anything about language type? In Proceedings of the 15th international Congress of phonetic Sciences, Barcelona (pp. 2693–2696).
Beckman, M. (1992). Evidence for speech rhythms across languages. In Y. Tohkura, E. Vatikiotis‐Bateson, & Y. Sagisaka (Eds.), Speech perception, production, and linguistic structure (pp. 458–463). Ohmsha.
Benuš, S., & Šimko, J. (2012). Rhythm and tempo in Slovak. In Proceedings of the 6th international conference on speech prosody, SP 2012.
Bhatara, A., Boll‐Avetisyan, N., Unger, A., Nazzi, T., & Höhle, B. (2013). Native language affects rhythmic grouping of speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(5), 3828–3843. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4823848
Bloch, B. (1950). Studies in Colloquial Japanese IV phonemics. Language, 26(1), 86. https://doi.org/10.2307/410409
Bosch, L., & Sebastián‐Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early Language Discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2(1), 29–49. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0201_3
Bosch, L., & Sebastián‐Gallés, N. (1997). Native‐language recognition abilities in 4‐month‐old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65(1), 33–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010‐0277(97)00040‐1
Burnham, D. K. (1986). Developmental loss of speech perception: Exposure to and experience with a first language. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 7(3), 207–239. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716400007542
Butler, J., Floccia, C., Goslin, J., & Panneton, R. (2011). Infants’ discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar accents in speech. Infancy, 16(4), 392–417. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532‐7078.2010.00050.x
Byers‐Heinlein, K., Burns, T. C., & Werker, J. F. (2010). The Roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21(3), 343–348. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609360758
Carbajal, M. J. (2018). Separation and acquisition of two languages in early childhood: A multidisciplinary approach. Doctoral dissertation. Universite de recherche Paris Sciences et Lettres.
Chládková, K., & Paillereau, N. (2020). The what and when of universal perception: A review of early speech sound acquisition. Language Learning, 70(4), 1136–1182. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12422
Chong, A. J., Vicenik, C., & Sundara, M. (2018). Intonation plays a role in Language Discrimination by infants. Infancy, 23(6), 795–819. Portico. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12257
Christophe, A., & Morton, J. (1998). Is Dutch native English? Linguistic analysis by 2‐month‐olds. Developmental Science, 1(2), 215–219. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467‐7687.00033
Christophe, A., Nespor, M., Teresa Guasti, M., & Van Ooyen, B. (2003). Prosodic structure and syntactic acquisition: The case of the head‐direction parameter. Developmental Science, 6(2), 211–220. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467‐7687.00273
Cooper, N., Cutler, A., & Wales, R. (2002). Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and non‐native listeners. Language and Speech, 45(3), 207–228. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309020450030101
Cumming, R. E. (2011). Perceptually informed quantification of speech rhythm in pairwise variability indices. Phonetica, 68(4), 256–277. https://doi.org/10.1159/000335416
Cutler, A., & Carter, D. M. (1987). The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech and Language, 2(3–4), 133–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885‐2308(87)90004‐0
Dankovičová, J., & Dellwo, V. (2007). Czech speech rhythm and the rhythm class hypothesis. In Proceedings of 16th ICPhS (pp. 1241–1244).
Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress‐timing and syllable‐timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11(1), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0095‐4470(19)30776‐4
Delattre, P. (1963). Comparing the prosodic features of English, German, Spanish and French. IRAL ‐ International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 1(1). https://doi.org/10.1515/iral.1963.1.1.193
Diehl, M., Panneton, R., Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (2006). Six‐month‐olds' perception of native speech accent. In Paper presented at the annual meeting of the XVth Biennial international conference on infant studies, Westin Miyako, Kyoto, Japan.
Frota, S., & Vigário, M. (2001). On the correlates of rhythmic distinctions: The European/Brazilian Portuguese case. Probus, 13(2). https://doi.org/10.1515/prbs.2001.005
Gasparini, L., Langus, A., Tsuji, S., & Boll‐Avetisyan, N. (2021). Quantifying the role of rhythm in infants’ language discrimination abilities: A meta‐analysis. Cognition, 213, 104757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104757
Gervain, J. (2018). Gateway to language: The perception of prosody at birth. Boundaries crossed, at the Interfaces of Morphosyntax. Phonology, Pragmatics and Semantics, 373–384. https://doi.org/10.1007/978‐3‐319‐90710‐9_23
Gervain, J., & Mehler, J. (2010). Speech perception and language acquisition in the first Year of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 191–218. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100408
Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Prosody cues word order in 7‐month‐old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4(1), 1490. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms2430
Gibbon, D., Klessa, K., & Bachan, J. (2014). Duration and speed of speech events: A selection of methods. Lingua Posnaniensis, 56(1), 59–83. https://doi.org/10.2478/linpo‐2014‐0004
Gleitman, L. R. (2020). The structural sources of Verb meanings. Sentence First, Arguments Afterward, 261–312. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780199828098.003.0009
Goedemans, R., & van der Hulst, H. (2009). StressTyp: A database for word accentual patterns in the world’s languages. In M. Everaert, S. Musgrave, & A. Dimitriadis (Eds.), The Use of Databases in cross‐Linguistics studies (pp. 235–282). Mouton de Gruyter.
Grabe, E., & Low, E. L. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Laboratory phonology (pp. 515–546). Mouton de Gruyter.
Granier‐Deferre, C., Ribeiro, A., Jacquet, A.‐Y., & Bassereau, S. (2011). Near‐term fetuses process temporal features of speech. Developmental Science, 14(2), 336–352. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2010.00978.x
Hagmann, L., & Dellwo, V. (2014). Listeners may rely on intonation to distinguish languages of different rhythm classes. Loquens, 1(1), e008. https://doi.org/10.3989/loquens.2014.008
Han, M. S. (1962). The feature of duration in Japanese. Onsei no kenkyuu, 10(1), 65–80.
Hayes, B. (1995). Metrical stress theory: Principles and case studies. In Proceedings of the Eleventh annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics (pp. 429–446). University of Chicago Press.
He, L. (2012). Syllabic intensity variations as quantification of speech rhythm: Evidence from both L1 and L2. In Q. Ma, H. Ding, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th international conference on speech prosody, Shanghai, 22‐26 May 2012 (pp. 466–469). Tongji University Press.
Höhle, B., Bijeljac‐Babic, R., & Nazzi, T. (2020). Variability and stability in early language acquisition: Comparing monolingual and bilingual infants' speech perception and word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 56–71. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000348
Houston, D., Santelmann, L., & Jusczyk, P. (2004). English‐learning infants’ segmentation of trisyllabic words from fluent speech. Language & Cognitive Processes, 19(1), 97–136. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000143
Inkelas, S. (1999). Exceptional stress‐attracting suffixes in Turkish: Representations versus the grammar. In R. Kager, H. van der Hulst, & W. Zonneveld (Eds.), The prosody‐Morphology Interface (pp. 134–187). Cambridge University Press.
Inkelas, S., & Orgun, C. (2003). Turkish stress: A review. Phonology, 20(01), 139–161. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0952675703004482
Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., & Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4), 2263–2271. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2973189
Johnson, E. K., & Braun, B. (2011). The role of intonation in language discrimination by 4.5‐month‐olds. Society for Research in Child Development.
Johnson, E. K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Ramus, F. (2003). The role of segmental information in Language Discrimination by English‐learning 5‐month‐olds. Unpublished.
Jun, S.‐Ah (1993). The phonetics and phonology of Korean prosody. PhD Dissertation. State University.
Kabak, B., & Vogel, I. (2001). The phonological word and stress assignment in Turkish. Phonology, 18(3), 315–360. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0952675701004201
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12577–12580. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705345104
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Brown, C. A., Lee, C. T., Cowperthwaite, B., Stutzman, S. S., Swansburg, M. L., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H.‐H., Zhang, K., & Wang, Z. (2009). Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002
Kitamura, C., Panneton, R., & Best, C. T. (2013). The development of language constancy: Attention to native versus Nonnative accents. Child Development, 84(5), 1686–1700. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12068
Kitamura, C., Panneton, R., Deihl, M., & Notley, A. (2006). Attuning to the native dialect: When more means less. In P. Warren & C. I. Watson (Eds.), Proceedings of the 11th Australasian international conference on speech science and Technology (pp. 124–129). Australian Speech Science and Technology Association.
Ladefoged, P. (1975). A course in phonetics. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Lea, W. A. (1974). Prosodic aids to speech recognition: IV. A general strategy for prosodically‐guided speech understanding. In Univac Report PX10791, St. Paul, Minnesota: Sperry univac.
Levi, S. (2005). Acoustic correlates of lexical accent in Turkish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 35(1), 73–97. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0025100305001921
Levi, S. V. (2018). Another bilingual advantage? Perception of talker‐voice information. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 523–536. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000153
Liu, L., & Kager, R. (2016). Perception of a native vowel contrast by Dutch monolingual and bilingual infants: A bilingual perceptual lead. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(3), 335–345. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006914566082
Low, E. L. (1998). Prosodic prominence in Singapore English. PhD thesis. University of Cambridge.
Major, R. C. (1981). Stress‐timing in Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Phonetics, 9(3), 343–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0095‐4470(19)30977‐5
Malisz, Z. (2013). Speech rhythm variability in Polish and English: A study of Interaction between rhythmic levels. PhD thesis. Adam Mickiewicz University.
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns' cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19(23), 1994–1997. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.064
Martinez‐Alvarez, A., Benavides‐Varela, S., Lapillonne, A., & Gervain, J. (2023). Newborns discriminate utterance‐level prosodic contours. Developmental Science, 26(2), e13304. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13304
Martinez‐Alvarez, A., Gervain, J., Koulaguina, E., Pons, F., & de Diego‐Balaguer, R. (2023). Prosodic cues enhance infants’ sensitivity to nonadjacent regularities. Science Advances, 9(15), eade4083. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.ade4083
May, L., Byers‐Heinlein, K., Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Language and the newborn brain: Does prenatal Language experience Shape the Neonate neural response to speech? Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00222
May, L., Gervain, J., Carreiras, M., & Werker, J. F. (2018). The specificity of the neural response to speech at birth. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12564. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12564
Mehler, J., & Christophe, A. (1995). Maturation and learning of language in the first year of life. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 943–954). Bradford Books.
Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Nazzi, T., & Dehaene‐Lambertz, G. (1996). Coping with linguistic diversity: The infant’s viewpoint. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 101–116). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel‐Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29(2), 143–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010‐0277(88)90035‐2
Mehler, J., & Nespor, M. (2004). Linguistic rhythm and the acquisition of language (Vol. 3, pp. 213–222). Oxford University Press.
Molnar, M., Gervain, J., & Carreiras, M. (2013). Within‐rhythm class native Language Discrimination abilities of Basque‐Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 Months of age. Infancy, 19(3), 326–337. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12041
Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two‐day‐olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 495–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163‐6383(93)80007‐u
Morgan, J. L. (1986). From simple input to complex grammar. MIT Press.
Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K. (Eds.) (2014)., Signal to syntax. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315806822
Mueller, J. L., Männel, C., & Friederici, A. D. (2015). Biological Preconditions for language development. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 650–655. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978‐0‐08‐097086‐8.23117‐6
Narayan, C. R. (2019). An acoustic perspective on 45 years of infant speech perception, Part 1: Consonants. Language and Linguistics Compass, 13(10). https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12352
Narayan, C. R. (2020). An acoustic perspective on 45 years of infant speech perception. II. Vowels and suprasegmentals. Language and Linguistics Compass, 14(5). https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12369
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096‐1523.24.3.756
Nazzi, T., Jusczyk, P. W., & Johnson, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by English‐learning 5‐month‐olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2698
Nazzi, T., & Ramus, F. (2003). Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants. Speech Communication, 41(1), 233–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167‐6393(02)00106‐1
Nespor, M. (1990). On the rhythm parameter in phonology. In I. M. Roca (Ed.), Logical issues in language acquisition (pp. 157–175). Foris.
Nespor, M., Shukla, M., & Mehler, J. (2011). Stress‐timed vs. Syllable‐timed languages. The Blackwell Companion to Phonology, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444335262.wbctp0048
Nespor, M., Shukla, M., Vijver Van de, R., Avesani, C., Schraudolf, H., & Donati, C. (2008). Different phrasal prominence realizations in VO et OV languages. Lingue E. Linguaggio, 2, 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1418/28093
Nespor, M., & Vogel, I. (1986). Prosodic phonology. Studies in Generative grammar, 28. Foris Publications.
Nolan, F., & Asu, E. L. (2009). The pairwise variability index and Coexisting rhythms in language. Phonetica, 66(1–2), 64–77. https://doi.org/10.1159/000208931
Nolan, F., & Jeon, H.‐S. (2014). Speech rhythm: A metaphor? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1658), 20130396. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0396
O'Connor, J. D. (1965). The perception of time intervals. Progress Report 2. Phonetics Laboratory, University College.
Paillereau, N., Podlipský, V. J., Smolík, F., Šimáčková, Š., & Chládková, K. (2021). The development of infants’ sensitivity to native versus non‐native rhythm. Infancy, 26(3), 423–441. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12395
Palková, Z. (2012). Rytmus řeči a verše v češtině. Česká Literatura, 60(3), 338–354. Retrieved June 28, 2021, from http://www.jstor.org/stable/42687868
Pike, K. L. (1945). The intonation of American English. University of Michigan Press.
Prieto, P., del Mar Vanrell, M., Astruc, L., Payne, E., & Post, B. (2012). Phonotactic and phrasal properties of speech rhythm. Evidence from Catalan, English, and Spanish. Speech Communication, 54(6), 681–702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.12.001
Ramus, F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns. Annual Review of Language Acquisition, 2, 85–115. https://doi.org/10.1075/arla.2.05ram
Ramus, F., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (2003). The psychological reality of rhythm class: Perceptual studies. In Proceedings of the 15th international Congress of phonetic Sciences (pp. 337–342). Barcelona.
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by Cotton‐Top Tamarin Monkeys. Science, 288(5464), 349–351. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5464.349
Ramus, F., & Mehler, J. (1999). Language identification with suprasegmental cues: A study based on speech resynthesis. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(1), 512–521. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.424522
Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 75(1), AD3–AD30. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010‐0277(00)00101‐3
Rossi, M. (1980). Le français, langue sans accent? In I. Fónagy & P. P. Léon (Eds.), L'accent en français contemporain (Vol. 15, pp. 13–51). Studia Phonetica.
Sansavini, A., Bertoncini, J., & Giovanelli, G. (1997). Newborns discriminate the rhythm of multisyllabic stressed words. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012‐1649.33.1.3
Seidl, A. (2007). Infants’ use and weighting of prosodic cues in clause segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(1), 24–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.10.004
Seidl, A., & Cristia, A. (2008). Developmental changes in the weighting of prosodic cues. Developmental Science, 4, 596–606. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐7687.2008.00704.x
Sezer, E. (1983). On non‐final stress in Turkish. Journal of Turkish Studies, 5, 61–69.
Shen, Y., & Peterson, G. G. (1962). Isochronism in English. University of Buffalo Studies in Linguistics ‐ Occasional papers, 9, 1–36.
Simões, A. (1991). Rhythmic patterns of the discourse in Mexican Spanish and Brazilian Portuguese. In Proceedings of the XIIth international Congress of phonetic Sciences, Aix‐en‐Provence (pp. 190–193).
Thorson, J. C. (2018). Chapter 4. The role of prosody in early word learning. The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition, 59–77. https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.23.04tho
Van Heuven, V. J., & Turk, A. (2020). Phonetic correlates of word and sentence stress. The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody, 149–165. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198832232.013.8
Van Leyden, K., & van Heuven, V. J. (1996). Lexical stress and spoken word recognition. Linguistics in the Netherlands 1996, 13, 159–170. https://doi.org/10.1075/avt.13.16ley
Vannasing, P., Flore, O., González‐Rankenberger, B., Tremblay, J., Paquette, N., Safi, D., Wallois, F., Lepore, F., Béland, R., Lassonde, M., & Gallagher, A. (2016). Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non‐native languages in one‐day‐old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia, 84, 63–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.038
Van Zanten, E., & Goedemans, R. W. N. (2007). A functional typology of Austronesian and Papuan stress systems. In V. J. van Heuven & E. van Zanten (Eds.), Prosody in Indonesian languages (pp. 63–88). LOT.
Volín, J. (2017). Appeal and disrepute of the so‐called global rhythm metrics. AUC PHILOLOGICA, 2017(3), 79–94. https://doi.org/10.14712/24646830.2017.35
Volín, J., & Skarnitzl, R. (2020). Accent‐groups vs. Stress‐groups in Czech clear and Conversational speech. Speech Prosody 2020. https://doi.org/10.21437/speechprosody.2020‐142
Wagner, A. (2014). (pp. 366–370).Description of Polish speech rhythm using rhythm metrics and the time‐delay approach: A comparative study.
Weissenborn, J. & Höhle, B. (Eds.) (2001)., Approaches to bootstrapping. Language acquisition and language Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.24
Werker, J. F., & Gervain, J. (2013). Speech perception in infancy: A Foundation for language acquisition. In The Oxford Handbook of developmental Psychology, Vol. 1: Body and Mind (pp. 908–925). https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.013.0031
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2005). Speech perception as a window for understanding plasticity and commitment in language systems of the brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 46(3), 233–251. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20060
White, L. (2014). Communicative function and prosodic form in speech timing. Speech Communication, 63–64, 38–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2014.04.003
White, L., Floccia, C., Goslin, J., & Butler, J. (2014). Utterance‐final lengthening is predictive of infants’ discrimination of English accents. Language Learning, 64(s2), 27–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12060
White, L., Luche, C. D., & Floccia, C. (2016). Five‐month‐old infants‘ discrimination of unfamiliar languages does not accord with “rhythm class.”. Speech Prosody 2016. https://doi.org/10.21437/speechprosody.2016‐116
White, L., & Mattys, S. L. (2007a). Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies. Journal of Phonetics, 35(4), 501–522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2007.02.003
White, L., & Mattys, S. L. (2007b). Rhythmic typology and variation in first and second languages (pp. 237–257). Current Issues in Linguistic Theory. https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.282.16whi
White, L., Mattys, S. L., & Wiget, L. (2012). Language categorization by adults is based on sensitivity to durational cues, not rhythm class. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 665–679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.12.010
White, L., Payne, E., & Mattys, S. L. (2009). Rhythmic and prosodic contrast in Venetian and Sicilian Italian (pp. 137–158). Current Issues in Linguistic Theory. https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.306.07whi
Zacharaki, K., & Sebastián‐Gallés, N. (2021). The ontogeny of early language discrimination: Beyond rhythm. Cognition, 104628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104628
Cristia, A., Minagawa‐Kawai, Y., Egorova, N., Gervain, J., Filippin, L., Cabrol, D., & Dupoux, E. (2014). Neural correlates of infant accent discrimination: an fNIRS study. Developmental Science, 17(4), 628–635.
Peña, M., Pittaluga, E., & Mehler, J. (2010). Language acquisition in premature and full‐term infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(8), 3823–3828.
Nacar Garcia, L., Guerrero‐Mosquera, C., Colomer, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2018). Evoked and oscillatory EEG activity differentiates language discrimination in young monolingual and bilingual infants. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 2770.