Violence exposure is associated with preference for masculine faces: evidence from Senegal
Jazyk angličtina Země Velká Británie, Anglie Médium print-electronic
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články
Grantová podpora
Grantová Agentura České Republiky
Akademie Věd České Republiky
PubMed
40068826
PubMed Central
PMC11896705
DOI
10.1098/rspb.2024.3105
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- Klíčová slova
- attractiveness, contextual preferences, evolutionary psychology, facial masculinity, trustworthiness, violence exposure,
- MeSH
- dospělí MeSH
- lidé MeSH
- mladiství MeSH
- mladý dospělý MeSH
- mužskost MeSH
- násilí MeSH
- obličej * MeSH
- výběrové chování MeSH
- vystavení násilí psychologie MeSH
- Check Tag
- dospělí MeSH
- lidé MeSH
- mladiství MeSH
- mladý dospělý MeSH
- mužské pohlaví MeSH
- ženské pohlaví MeSH
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
- Geografické názvy
- Senegal MeSH
It has been suggested that in threatening environments, both women and men should prefer more masculine men as romantic and coalition partners, respectively. Empirical evidence for this hypothesis has been weak or inconsistent, primarily because most experimental research has focused on elevating the perceived danger from other men through virtual threats. This study investigates whether personal experience with violence predicts the preference for masculine features in 326 Senegalese participants presented with pairs of manipulated facial photographs of West African men (one more feminine, one more masculine) and asked to indicate which face is more attractive (to women) or more trustworthy (to men). The findings reveal a strong association between violence exposure and facial feature preferences. Those who experienced (particularly physical) violence showed a higher preference for masculinized faces (up to 95% in women, 82% in men) compared to the baseline (57% in women, 63% in men). This difference is proposed to reflect an adaptive strategy of prioritizing physical protection in settings with a higher incidence of violent confrontations. Much of the variance can be found between groups. The direct effect of experienced violence diminishes over time, which suggests a dynamic interplay between innate predispositions and environmental influences on aesthetic preferences.
Center for Theoretical Study Charles University Praha Czech Republic
Département d'Histoire Université Cheikh Anta Diop de Dakar Fann Dakar Senegal
Department of Anthropology and Human Genetics Charles University Praha Czech Republic
Department of Philosophy and History of Science Charles University Praha Czech Republic
Faculté des Humanités Université de Lille Lille Hauts de France France
Institute of Archaeology Czech Academy of Sciences Praha Czech Republic
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz LA, Simmons LW. 2003. Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Biol. Lett. 270, S93–5. (10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023) PubMed DOI PMC
Weston EM, Friday AE, Liò P. 2007. Biometric evidence that sexual selection has shaped the hominin face. PLoS One 2, e710. (10.1371/journal.pone.0000710) PubMed DOI PMC
Haselhuhn MP, Wong EM. 2012. Bad to the bone: facial structure predicts unethical behaviour. Proc. R. Soc.B 279, 571–576. (10.1098/rspb.2011.1193) PubMed DOI PMC
Carré JM, McCormick CM, Mondloch CJ. 2009. Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1194–1198. (10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.x) PubMed DOI
Fiala V, Tureček P, Akoko R, Pokorný Š, Kleisner K. 2022. Africans and Europeans differ in their facial perception of dominance and sex-typicality: a multidimensional bayesian approach. Sci. Rep. 12, 6821. (10.1038/s41598-022-10646-6) PubMed DOI PMC
Marcinkowska U. 2019. Women’s preferences for men’s facial masculinity are strongest under favorable ecological conditions. Sci. Rep. 9, 3387. (10.1038/s41598-019-39350-8) PubMed DOI PMC
Short LA, Mondloch CJ, McCormick CM, Carré JM, Ma R, Fu G, Lee K. 2012. Detection of propensity for aggression based on facial structure irrespective of face race. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 121–129. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.07.002) PubMed DOI PMC
Dong J, Leger K, Shiramizu V, Marcinkowska U, Lee A, Jones B. 2023. The importance of face-shape masculinity for perceptions of male dominance depends on study design. Sci. Rep. 13, 12620. (10.1038/s41598-023-39912-x) PubMed DOI PMC
Felson RB. 1996. Big people hit little people: sex differences in physical power and interpersonal violence. Criminology 34, 433–452. (10.1111/j.1745-9125.1996.tb01214.x) DOI
Little AC, Třebický V, Havlíček J, Roberts SC, Kleisner K. 2015. Human perception of fighting ability: facial cues predict winners and losers in mixed martial arts fights. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1470–1475. (10.1093/beheco/arv089) DOI
Trebicky V, Havlícek J, Roberts SC, Little AC, Kleisner K. 2013. Perceived aggressiveness predicts fighting performance in mixed-martial-arts fighters. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1664–1672. (10.1177/0956797613477117) PubMed DOI
Třebický V, Fialová J, Kleisner K, Roberts S, Little A, Havlíček J. 2015. Further evidence for links between facial width‐to‐height ratio and fighting success: commentary on zilioli et al. (2014). Aggress. Behav. 41, 331–334. (10.1002/ab.21559) PubMed DOI
Butovskaya ML, Windhager S, Karelin D, Mezentseva A, Schaefer K, Fink B. 2018. Associations of physical strength with facial shape in an african pastoralist society, the maasai of Northern Tanzania. PLoS One 13, e0197738. (10.1371/journal.pone.0197738) PubMed DOI PMC
Butovskaya M, Rostovtseva V, Mezentseva A. 2022. Facial and body sexual dimorphism are not interconnected in the maasai. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 41, 3. (10.1186/s40101-021-00276-8) PubMed DOI PMC
Mezentseva AA, Rostovtseva VV, Butovskaya ML. 2024. Facial cues to physical strength are not always associated with facial masculinity: comparative study of Europeans and Southern Siberians. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 36, e23974. (10.1002/ajhb.23974) PubMed DOI
Holzleitner IJ, Perrett DI. 2016. Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 217–229. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.11.004) DOI
Windhager S, Schaefer K, Fink B. 2011. Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 23, 805–814. (10.1002/ajhb.21219) PubMed DOI
Saribay SA, Tureček P, Paluch R, Kleisner K. 2021. Differential effects of resource scarcity and pathogen prevalence on heterosexual women’s facial masculinity preferences. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3, e48. (10.1017/ehs.2021.42) PubMed DOI PMC
Whitehouse A. 2015. Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20151351. (10.1098/rspb.2015.1351) PubMed DOI PMC
Pound N, Penton-Voak IS, Surridge AK. 2009. Testosterone responses to competition in men are related to facial masculinity. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 153–159. (10.1098/rspb.2008.0990) PubMed DOI PMC
Geniole SN, Bird BM, McVittie JS, Purcell RB, Archer J, Carré JM. 2020. Is testosterone linked to human aggression? A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between baseline, dynamic, and manipulated testosterone on human aggression. Horm. Behav. 123, 104644. (10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104644) PubMed DOI
Edelstein R, Van Anders S, Chopik W, Goldey K, Wardecker B. 2014. Dyadic associations between testosterone and relationship quality in couples. Horm. Behav. 65, 401–407. (10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.03.003) PubMed DOI
Gangestad SW, Simpson JA. 2000. The evolution of human mating: trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 573–587. (10.1017/s0140525x0000337x) PubMed DOI
Lu HJ, Zhu XQ, Chang L. 2015. Good genes, good providers, and good fathers: economic development involved in how women select a mate. Evol.Behav. Sci. 9, 215–228. (10.1037/ebs0000048) DOI
Mazur A, Booth A. 1998. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 353–363. (10.1017/S0140525X98001228) PubMed DOI
Swaddle JP, Reierson GW. 2002. Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 2285–2289. (10.1098/rspb.2002.2165) PubMed DOI PMC
DeBruine L, et al. . 2006. Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner’s masculinity. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 1355–1360. (10.1098/rspb.2005.3445) PubMed DOI PMC
Johnston VS, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K. 2001. Male facial attractiveness evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design Victor. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 251–267. (10.1145/3011286.3011307) DOI
Penton-Voak IS, Jones BC, Little AC, Baker S, Tiddeman B, Burt DM, Perrett DI. 2001. Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 1617–1623. (10.1098/rspb.2001.1703) PubMed DOI PMC
Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM, Henzi SP, Castles DL, Akamatsu S. 1998. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature 394, 884–887. (10.1038/29772) PubMed DOI
Todorov A, Said CC, Verosky SC. 2011. Personality impressions from facial appearance. In Oxford handbook of face perception. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Brooks R, Scott IM, Maklakov AA, Kasumovic MM, Clark AP, Penton-Voak IS. 2011. National income inequality predicts women’s preferences for masculinized faces better than health does. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 810–812. (10.1098/rspb.2010.0964) PubMed DOI PMC
DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Crawford JR, Welling LLM, Little AC. 2010. The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for masculinized male faces. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2405–2410. (10.1098/rspb.2009.2184) PubMed DOI PMC
Little AC, Cohen DL, Jones BC, Belsky J. 2007. Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 967–973. (10.1007/s00265-006-0325-7) DOI
Little AC, DeBruine LM, Jones BC. 2011. Exposure to visual cues of pathogen contagion changes preferences for masculinity and symmetry in opposite-sex faces. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 2032–2039. (10.1098/rspb.2010.1925) PubMed DOI PMC
Pisanski K, Feinberg DR. 2013. Cross-cultural variation in mate preferences for averageness, symmetry, body size, and masculinity. Cross Cult. Res. 47, 162–197. (10.1177/1069397112471806) DOI
Little AC, DeBruine LM, Jones BC. 2013. Environment contingent preferences: exposure to visual cues of direct male–male competition and wealth increase women’s preferences for masculinity in male faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 34, 193–200. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.11.008) DOI
Sacco DF, Lustgraaf C, Brown M, Young SG. 2015. Activation of self-protection threat increases women’s preferences for dominance in male faces. Hum. Ethol. Bull. 30, 23–31. (10.22330/heb/304/023-031) DOI
Borras-Guevara ML, Batres C, Perrett DI. 2017. Aggressor or protector? Experiences and perceptions of violence predict preferences for masculinity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 481–489. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.03.004) DOI
Watkins CD, Jones BC. 2016. Competition-related factors directly influence preferences for facial cues of dominance in allies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 2071–2079. (10.1007/s00265-016-2211-2) PubMed DOI PMC
Singleton RA, Vacca J. 2007. Interpersonal competition in friendships. Sex Roles 57, 617–627. (10.1007/s11199-007-9298-x) DOI
Rostovtseva VVButovskaya MLMezentseva AA, Weissing FJ. 2023. Effects of sex and sex-related facial traits on trust and trustworthiness: an experimental study. Front. Psychol. 13, 925601. (10.3389/fpsyg.2022.925601) PubMed DOI PMC
Hu Y, Abbasi NUH, Zhang Y, Chen H. 2018. The effect of target sex, sexual dimorphism, and facial attractiveness on perceptions of target attractiveness and trustworthiness. Front. Psychol. 9, 942. (10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00942) PubMed DOI PMC
Lee AJ, Wright MJ, Martin NG, Keller MC, Zietsch BP. 2017. Facial trustworthiness is associated with heritable aspects of face shape. Adapt. Human Behav. Physiol. 3, 351–364. (10.1007/s40750-017-0073-0) DOI
Dixson BJW, Nelson NL, Moses E, Lee AJ, Pegna AJ. 2024. Perceptions of facial trustworthiness and dominance modulate early neural responses to male facial sexual dimorphism. Evol. Hum. Behav. 45, 106629. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106629) DOI
Luo X, Song J, Guan J, Wang X, Chen L. 2024. Influence of facial dimorphism on interpersonal trust: weighing warmth and competence traits in different trust situations. Curr. Psychol. 43, 2158–2172. (10.1007/s12144-023-04472-w) DOI
Little AC, Burriss RP, Jones BC, Roberts SC. 2007. Facial appearance affects voting decisions. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 18–27. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.09.002) DOI
Laustsen L, Petersen MB. 2015. Does a competent leader make a good friend? Conflict, ideology and the psychologies of friendship and followership. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 286–293. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.001) DOI
Hasel MC, Grover SL. 2017. An integrative model of trust and leadership. LODJ 38, 849–867. (10.1108/LODJ-12-2015-0293) DOI
Van ’T Wout M, Sanfey A. 2008. Friend or foe: the effect of implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision-making. Cognition 108, 796–803. (10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.002) PubMed DOI
Fiala V, Třebický V, Pazhoohi F, Leongómez J, Tureček P, Saribay S, Akoko R, Kleisner K. 2021. Facial attractiveness and preference of sexual dimorphism: a comparison across five populations. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3, e38. (10.1017/ehs.2021.33) PubMed DOI PMC
Rohlf F. 2015. The tps series of software. Hystrix Ital. J. Mammal 26, 9–12. (10.4404/hystrix-26.1-11264) DOI
Ethnologue . 2000. Ethnologue: languages of the world. Dallas, TX: SIL International.
Blench R. 2006. Archaeology, language, and the african past. Oxford, UK: Altamira Press.
Kulichová I, Fernandes V, Deme A, Nováčková J, Stenzl V, Novelletto A, Pereira L, Černý V. 2017. Internal diversification of non‐Sub‐Saharan haplogroups in Sahelian. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 164, 424–434. (10.1002/ajpa.23285) PubMed DOI
Vicente M, Priehodová E, Diallo I, Podgorná E, Poloni ES, Černý V, Schlebusch CM. 2019. Population history and genetic adaptation of the Fulani nomads: inferences from genome-wide data and the lactase persistence trait. BMC Genomics 20, 915. (10.1186/s12864-019-6296-7) PubMed DOI PMC
Madzimbalale FC, Khoza LB. 2010. Experiences of physical violence by women living with intimate partners. Curationis 33, 25–32. (10.4102/curationis.v33i2.1080) PubMed DOI
Borras-Guevara ML, Batres C, Perrett DI. 2017. Domestic violence shapes Colombian women’s partner choices. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71, 175. (10.1007/s00265-017-2405-2) PubMed DOI PMC
Spedini G, Destro‐Bisol G, Mondovì S, Kaptué L, Taglioli L, Paoli G. 1999. The peopling of sub‐saharan Africa: the case study of cameroon. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 110, 143–162. (10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199910)110:23.0.CO;2-J) PubMed DOI
Cazes MH. 1990. Endogamy among the dogon of boni. Mali J. Biosoc. Sci. 22, 85–99. (10.1017/s002193200001840x) PubMed DOI
Hampshire KR, Smith MT. 2001. Consanguineous marriage among the fulani. Hum. Biol. 597–603. (10.1353/hub.2001.0051) PubMed DOI
Dupire M. 1972. L’incohérente endogamie des sociétés peul: une analyse comparative. Eur. J. Sociol. Eur. De Sociol. 13, 3–17.
Gessain M. 1963. Etude socio-démographique du mariage chez les Coniagui et les Bassari. Cah. Du Cent. De Rech. Anthropol. 2, 124–222.
Barrière O, Barrière C. 2005. Bassari: de l’ocre à la lumière. Paris, France: Romain Pages.
Little AC, Jones BC, Penton-Voak IS, Burt DM, Perrett DI. 2002. Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 1095–1100. (10.1098/rspb.2002.1984) PubMed DOI PMC
Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. 1999. The scent of symmetry: a human sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 175–201. (10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00005-7) DOI
Fink B, Neave N, Seydel H. 2007. Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 19, 82–87. (10.1002/ajhb.20583) PubMed DOI
Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. 2006. Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 131–144. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001) DOI
Boothroyd LG, Scott I, Gray AW, Coombes CI, Pound N. 2013. Male facial masculinity as a cue to health outcomes. Evol. Psychol. 11, 1044–1058. (10.1177/147470491301100508) PubMed DOI PMC
Zaidi AA, White JD, Mattern BC, Liebowitz CR, Puts DA, Claes P, Shriver MD. 2019. Facial masculinity does not appear to be a condition-dependent male ornament and does not reflect MHC heterozygosity in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 1633–1638. (10.1073/pnas.1808659116) PubMed DOI PMC
Boothroyd LG, Jones BC, Burt DM, DeBruine LM, Perrett DI. 2008. Facial correlates of sociosexuality. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 211–218. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.009) DOI
Lidborg L, Cross C, Boothroyd L. 2022. A meta-analysis of the association between male dimorphism and fitness outcomes in humans. eLife 11, e65031. (10.7554/eLife.65031) PubMed DOI PMC
Palumbo S, Mariotti V, Iofrida C, Pellegrini S. 2018. Genes and aggressive behavior: epigenetic mechanisms underlying individual susceptibility to aversive environments. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 117. (10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00117) PubMed DOI PMC
Leye MM, Sougou NM, Faye A, Seck I, Dia AT. 2019. People’s perceptions of violence against women in Senegal. Sante Publique 31, 581–590. (10.3917/spub.194.0581) PubMed DOI
Werwie TR, Hildon ZJL, Camara AD, Mbengue OK, Vondrasek C, Mbaye M, Mills H, Kumoji K, Babalola S. 2019. Gender-based violence in Senegal: its catalysts and connections from a community perspective. J. Fam. Violence 34, 769–780. (10.1007/s10896-019-00058-y) DOI
Alharbi SAH, Holzleitner IJ, Lee AJ, Saribay SA, Jones BC. 2021. Facial masculinity increases perceptions of men’s age, but not perceptions of their health: data from an Arab sample. Evol. Psychol. Sci 7, 184–188. (10.1007/s40806-020-00263-9) DOI
Alharbi SAH, Holzleitner IJ, Saribay SA, Jones BC, Lee AJ. 2021. Does self-rated attractiveness predict women’s preferences for facial masculinity? Data from an arab sample. Adapt. Human Behav. Physiol. 7, 105–113. (10.1007/s40750-021-00163-7) DOI
Butovskaya M, Rostovstseva V, Mezentseva A, Kavina A, Rizwan M, Shi Y, Vilimek V, Davletshin A. 2024. Cross-cultural perception of strength, attractiveness, aggressiveness and helpfulness of Maasai male faces calibrated to handgrip strength. Sci. Rep. 14, 5880. (10.1038/s41598-024-56607-z) PubMed DOI PMC
Coetzee V, Greeff JM, Stephen ID, Perrett DI. 2014. Cross-cultural agreement in facial attractiveness preferences: the role of ethnicity and gender. PLoS One 9, e99629. (10.1371/journal.pone.0099629) PubMed DOI PMC
Jones D, Hill K. 1993. Criteria of facial attractiveness in five populations. Hum. Nat. 4, 271–296. (10.1007/BF02692202) PubMed DOI
Phalane K, Tribe C, Steel H, Cholo M, Coetzee V. 2017. Facial appearance reveals immunity in African men. Sci. Rep. 7, 7443. (10.1038/s41598-017-08015-9) PubMed DOI PMC
Tureček P. 2025. Violence is associated with preference for masculine faces. OSF. See 10.17605/OSF.IO/4K68N. PubMed DOI
Tureček P, Černý V, Diallo MY, Cissé N, Pokorný Š, Kleisner K. 2025. Supplementary material from: Violence exposure is associated with preference for masculine faces: evidence from Senegal. Figshare (10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7684185) PubMed DOI
Violence exposure is associated with preference for masculine faces: evidence from Senegal