Violence exposure is associated with preference for masculine faces: evidence from Senegal

. 2025 Mar ; 292 (2042) : 20243105. [epub] 20250312

Jazyk angličtina Země Velká Británie, Anglie Médium print-electronic

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid40068826

Grantová podpora
Grantová Agentura České Republiky
Akademie Věd České Republiky

It has been suggested that in threatening environments, both women and men should prefer more masculine men as romantic and coalition partners, respectively. Empirical evidence for this hypothesis has been weak or inconsistent, primarily because most experimental research has focused on elevating the perceived danger from other men through virtual threats. This study investigates whether personal experience with violence predicts the preference for masculine features in 326 Senegalese participants presented with pairs of manipulated facial photographs of West African men (one more feminine, one more masculine) and asked to indicate which face is more attractive (to women) or more trustworthy (to men). The findings reveal a strong association between violence exposure and facial feature preferences. Those who experienced (particularly physical) violence showed a higher preference for masculinized faces (up to 95% in women, 82% in men) compared to the baseline (57% in women, 63% in men). This difference is proposed to reflect an adaptive strategy of prioritizing physical protection in settings with a higher incidence of violent confrontations. Much of the variance can be found between groups. The direct effect of experienced violence diminishes over time, which suggests a dynamic interplay between innate predispositions and environmental influences on aesthetic preferences.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz LA, Simmons LW. 2003. Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Biol. Lett. 270, S93–5. (10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023) PubMed DOI PMC

Weston EM, Friday AE, Liò P. 2007. Biometric evidence that sexual selection has shaped the hominin face. PLoS One 2, e710. (10.1371/journal.pone.0000710) PubMed DOI PMC

Haselhuhn MP, Wong EM. 2012. Bad to the bone: facial structure predicts unethical behaviour. Proc. R. Soc.B 279, 571–576. (10.1098/rspb.2011.1193) PubMed DOI PMC

Carré JM, McCormick CM, Mondloch CJ. 2009. Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1194–1198. (10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.x) PubMed DOI

Fiala V, Tureček P, Akoko R, Pokorný Š, Kleisner K. 2022. Africans and Europeans differ in their facial perception of dominance and sex-typicality: a multidimensional bayesian approach. Sci. Rep. 12, 6821. (10.1038/s41598-022-10646-6) PubMed DOI PMC

Marcinkowska U. 2019. Women’s preferences for men’s facial masculinity are strongest under favorable ecological conditions. Sci. Rep. 9, 3387. (10.1038/s41598-019-39350-8) PubMed DOI PMC

Short LA, Mondloch CJ, McCormick CM, Carré JM, Ma R, Fu G, Lee K. 2012. Detection of propensity for aggression based on facial structure irrespective of face race. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 121–129. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.07.002) PubMed DOI PMC

Dong J, Leger K, Shiramizu V, Marcinkowska U, Lee A, Jones B. 2023. The importance of face-shape masculinity for perceptions of male dominance depends on study design. Sci. Rep. 13, 12620. (10.1038/s41598-023-39912-x) PubMed DOI PMC

Felson RB. 1996. Big people hit little people: sex differences in physical power and interpersonal violence. Criminology 34, 433–452. (10.1111/j.1745-9125.1996.tb01214.x) DOI

Little AC, Třebický V, Havlíček J, Roberts SC, Kleisner K. 2015. Human perception of fighting ability: facial cues predict winners and losers in mixed martial arts fights. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1470–1475. (10.1093/beheco/arv089) DOI

Trebicky V, Havlícek J, Roberts SC, Little AC, Kleisner K. 2013. Perceived aggressiveness predicts fighting performance in mixed-martial-arts fighters. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1664–1672. (10.1177/0956797613477117) PubMed DOI

Třebický V, Fialová J, Kleisner K, Roberts S, Little A, Havlíček J. 2015. Further evidence for links between facial width‐to‐height ratio and fighting success: commentary on zilioli et al. (2014). Aggress. Behav. 41, 331–334. (10.1002/ab.21559) PubMed DOI

Butovskaya ML, Windhager S, Karelin D, Mezentseva A, Schaefer K, Fink B. 2018. Associations of physical strength with facial shape in an african pastoralist society, the maasai of Northern Tanzania. PLoS One 13, e0197738. (10.1371/journal.pone.0197738) PubMed DOI PMC

Butovskaya M, Rostovtseva V, Mezentseva A. 2022. Facial and body sexual dimorphism are not interconnected in the maasai. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 41, 3. (10.1186/s40101-021-00276-8) PubMed DOI PMC

Mezentseva AA, Rostovtseva VV, Butovskaya ML. 2024. Facial cues to physical strength are not always associated with facial masculinity: comparative study of Europeans and Southern Siberians. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 36, e23974. (10.1002/ajhb.23974) PubMed DOI

Holzleitner IJ, Perrett DI. 2016. Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 217–229. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.11.004) DOI

Windhager S, Schaefer K, Fink B. 2011. Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 23, 805–814. (10.1002/ajhb.21219) PubMed DOI

Saribay SA, Tureček P, Paluch R, Kleisner K. 2021. Differential effects of resource scarcity and pathogen prevalence on heterosexual women’s facial masculinity preferences. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3, e48. (10.1017/ehs.2021.42) PubMed DOI PMC

Whitehouse A. 2015. Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20151351. (10.1098/rspb.2015.1351) PubMed DOI PMC

Pound N, Penton-Voak IS, Surridge AK. 2009. Testosterone responses to competition in men are related to facial masculinity. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 153–159. (10.1098/rspb.2008.0990) PubMed DOI PMC

Geniole SN, Bird BM, McVittie JS, Purcell RB, Archer J, Carré JM. 2020. Is testosterone linked to human aggression? A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between baseline, dynamic, and manipulated testosterone on human aggression. Horm. Behav. 123, 104644. (10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104644) PubMed DOI

Edelstein R, Van Anders S, Chopik W, Goldey K, Wardecker B. 2014. Dyadic associations between testosterone and relationship quality in couples. Horm. Behav. 65, 401–407. (10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.03.003) PubMed DOI

Gangestad SW, Simpson JA. 2000. The evolution of human mating: trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 573–587. (10.1017/s0140525x0000337x) PubMed DOI

Lu HJ, Zhu XQ, Chang L. 2015. Good genes, good providers, and good fathers: economic development involved in how women select a mate. Evol.Behav. Sci. 9, 215–228. (10.1037/ebs0000048) DOI

Mazur A, Booth A. 1998. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 353–363. (10.1017/S0140525X98001228) PubMed DOI

Swaddle JP, Reierson GW. 2002. Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 2285–2289. (10.1098/rspb.2002.2165) PubMed DOI PMC

DeBruine L, et al. . 2006. Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner’s masculinity. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 1355–1360. (10.1098/rspb.2005.3445) PubMed DOI PMC

Johnston VS, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K. 2001. Male facial attractiveness evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design Victor. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 251–267. (10.1145/3011286.3011307) DOI

Penton-Voak IS, Jones BC, Little AC, Baker S, Tiddeman B, Burt DM, Perrett DI. 2001. Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 1617–1623. (10.1098/rspb.2001.1703) PubMed DOI PMC

Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM, Henzi SP, Castles DL, Akamatsu S. 1998. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature 394, 884–887. (10.1038/29772) PubMed DOI

Todorov A, Said CC, Verosky SC. 2011. Personality impressions from facial appearance. In Oxford handbook of face perception. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Brooks R, Scott IM, Maklakov AA, Kasumovic MM, Clark AP, Penton-Voak IS. 2011. National income inequality predicts women’s preferences for masculinized faces better than health does. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 810–812. (10.1098/rspb.2010.0964) PubMed DOI PMC

DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Crawford JR, Welling LLM, Little AC. 2010. The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for masculinized male faces. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2405–2410. (10.1098/rspb.2009.2184) PubMed DOI PMC

Little AC, Cohen DL, Jones BC, Belsky J. 2007. Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 967–973. (10.1007/s00265-006-0325-7) DOI

Little AC, DeBruine LM, Jones BC. 2011. Exposure to visual cues of pathogen contagion changes preferences for masculinity and symmetry in opposite-sex faces. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 2032–2039. (10.1098/rspb.2010.1925) PubMed DOI PMC

Pisanski K, Feinberg DR. 2013. Cross-cultural variation in mate preferences for averageness, symmetry, body size, and masculinity. Cross Cult. Res. 47, 162–197. (10.1177/1069397112471806) DOI

Little AC, DeBruine LM, Jones BC. 2013. Environment contingent preferences: exposure to visual cues of direct male–male competition and wealth increase women’s preferences for masculinity in male faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 34, 193–200. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.11.008) DOI

Sacco DF, Lustgraaf C, Brown M, Young SG. 2015. Activation of self-protection threat increases women’s preferences for dominance in male faces. Hum. Ethol. Bull. 30, 23–31. (10.22330/heb/304/023-031) DOI

Borras-Guevara ML, Batres C, Perrett DI. 2017. Aggressor or protector? Experiences and perceptions of violence predict preferences for masculinity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 481–489. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.03.004) DOI

Watkins CD, Jones BC. 2016. Competition-related factors directly influence preferences for facial cues of dominance in allies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 2071–2079. (10.1007/s00265-016-2211-2) PubMed DOI PMC

Singleton RA, Vacca J. 2007. Interpersonal competition in friendships. Sex Roles 57, 617–627. (10.1007/s11199-007-9298-x) DOI

Rostovtseva VVButovskaya MLMezentseva AA, Weissing FJ. 2023. Effects of sex and sex-related facial traits on trust and trustworthiness: an experimental study. Front. Psychol. 13, 925601. (10.3389/fpsyg.2022.925601) PubMed DOI PMC

Hu Y, Abbasi NUH, Zhang Y, Chen H. 2018. The effect of target sex, sexual dimorphism, and facial attractiveness on perceptions of target attractiveness and trustworthiness. Front. Psychol. 9, 942. (10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00942) PubMed DOI PMC

Lee AJ, Wright MJ, Martin NG, Keller MC, Zietsch BP. 2017. Facial trustworthiness is associated with heritable aspects of face shape. Adapt. Human Behav. Physiol. 3, 351–364. (10.1007/s40750-017-0073-0) DOI

Dixson BJW, Nelson NL, Moses E, Lee AJ, Pegna AJ. 2024. Perceptions of facial trustworthiness and dominance modulate early neural responses to male facial sexual dimorphism. Evol. Hum. Behav. 45, 106629. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106629) DOI

Luo X, Song J, Guan J, Wang X, Chen L. 2024. Influence of facial dimorphism on interpersonal trust: weighing warmth and competence traits in different trust situations. Curr. Psychol. 43, 2158–2172. (10.1007/s12144-023-04472-w) DOI

Little AC, Burriss RP, Jones BC, Roberts SC. 2007. Facial appearance affects voting decisions. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 18–27. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.09.002) DOI

Laustsen L, Petersen MB. 2015. Does a competent leader make a good friend? Conflict, ideology and the psychologies of friendship and followership. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 286–293. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.001) DOI

Hasel MC, Grover SL. 2017. An integrative model of trust and leadership. LODJ 38, 849–867. (10.1108/LODJ-12-2015-0293) DOI

Van ’T Wout M, Sanfey A. 2008. Friend or foe: the effect of implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision-making. Cognition 108, 796–803. (10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.002) PubMed DOI

Fiala V, Třebický V, Pazhoohi F, Leongómez J, Tureček P, Saribay S, Akoko R, Kleisner K. 2021. Facial attractiveness and preference of sexual dimorphism: a comparison across five populations. Evol. Hum. Sci. 3, e38. (10.1017/ehs.2021.33) PubMed DOI PMC

Rohlf F. 2015. The tps series of software. Hystrix Ital. J. Mammal 26, 9–12. (10.4404/hystrix-26.1-11264) DOI

Ethnologue . 2000. Ethnologue: languages of the world. Dallas, TX: SIL International.

Blench R. 2006. Archaeology, language, and the african past. Oxford, UK: Altamira Press.

Kulichová I, Fernandes V, Deme A, Nováčková J, Stenzl V, Novelletto A, Pereira L, Černý V. 2017. Internal diversification of non‐Sub‐Saharan haplogroups in Sahelian. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 164, 424–434. (10.1002/ajpa.23285) PubMed DOI

Vicente M, Priehodová E, Diallo I, Podgorná E, Poloni ES, Černý V, Schlebusch CM. 2019. Population history and genetic adaptation of the Fulani nomads: inferences from genome-wide data and the lactase persistence trait. BMC Genomics 20, 915. (10.1186/s12864-019-6296-7) PubMed DOI PMC

Madzimbalale FC, Khoza LB. 2010. Experiences of physical violence by women living with intimate partners. Curationis 33, 25–32. (10.4102/curationis.v33i2.1080) PubMed DOI

Borras-Guevara ML, Batres C, Perrett DI. 2017. Domestic violence shapes Colombian women’s partner choices. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71, 175. (10.1007/s00265-017-2405-2) PubMed DOI PMC

Spedini G, Destro‐Bisol G, Mondovì S, Kaptué L, Taglioli L, Paoli G. 1999. The peopling of sub‐saharan Africa: the case study of cameroon. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 110, 143–162. (10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199910)110:23.0.CO;2-J) PubMed DOI

Cazes MH. 1990. Endogamy among the dogon of boni. Mali J. Biosoc. Sci. 22, 85–99. (10.1017/s002193200001840x) PubMed DOI

Hampshire KR, Smith MT. 2001. Consanguineous marriage among the fulani. Hum. Biol. 597–603. (10.1353/hub.2001.0051) PubMed DOI

Dupire M. 1972. L’incohérente endogamie des sociétés peul: une analyse comparative. Eur. J. Sociol. Eur. De Sociol. 13, 3–17.

Gessain M. 1963. Etude socio-démographique du mariage chez les Coniagui et les Bassari. Cah. Du Cent. De Rech. Anthropol. 2, 124–222.

Barrière O, Barrière C. 2005. Bassari: de l’ocre à la lumière. Paris, France: Romain Pages.

Little AC, Jones BC, Penton-Voak IS, Burt DM, Perrett DI. 2002. Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 1095–1100. (10.1098/rspb.2002.1984) PubMed DOI PMC

Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. 1999. The scent of symmetry: a human sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 175–201. (10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00005-7) DOI

Fink B, Neave N, Seydel H. 2007. Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 19, 82–87. (10.1002/ajhb.20583) PubMed DOI

Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. 2006. Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 131–144. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001) DOI

Boothroyd LG, Scott I, Gray AW, Coombes CI, Pound N. 2013. Male facial masculinity as a cue to health outcomes. Evol. Psychol. 11, 1044–1058. (10.1177/147470491301100508) PubMed DOI PMC

Zaidi AA, White JD, Mattern BC, Liebowitz CR, Puts DA, Claes P, Shriver MD. 2019. Facial masculinity does not appear to be a condition-dependent male ornament and does not reflect MHC heterozygosity in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 1633–1638. (10.1073/pnas.1808659116) PubMed DOI PMC

Boothroyd LG, Jones BC, Burt DM, DeBruine LM, Perrett DI. 2008. Facial correlates of sociosexuality. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 211–218. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.009) DOI

Lidborg L, Cross C, Boothroyd L. 2022. A meta-analysis of the association between male dimorphism and fitness outcomes in humans. eLife 11, e65031. (10.7554/eLife.65031) PubMed DOI PMC

Palumbo S, Mariotti V, Iofrida C, Pellegrini S. 2018. Genes and aggressive behavior: epigenetic mechanisms underlying individual susceptibility to aversive environments. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 117. (10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00117) PubMed DOI PMC

Leye MM, Sougou NM, Faye A, Seck I, Dia AT. 2019. People’s perceptions of violence against women in Senegal. Sante Publique 31, 581–590. (10.3917/spub.194.0581) PubMed DOI

Werwie TR, Hildon ZJL, Camara AD, Mbengue OK, Vondrasek C, Mbaye M, Mills H, Kumoji K, Babalola S. 2019. Gender-based violence in Senegal: its catalysts and connections from a community perspective. J. Fam. Violence 34, 769–780. (10.1007/s10896-019-00058-y) DOI

Alharbi SAH, Holzleitner IJ, Lee AJ, Saribay SA, Jones BC. 2021. Facial masculinity increases perceptions of men’s age, but not perceptions of their health: data from an Arab sample. Evol. Psychol. Sci 7, 184–188. (10.1007/s40806-020-00263-9) DOI

Alharbi SAH, Holzleitner IJ, Saribay SA, Jones BC, Lee AJ. 2021. Does self-rated attractiveness predict women’s preferences for facial masculinity? Data from an arab sample. Adapt. Human Behav. Physiol. 7, 105–113. (10.1007/s40750-021-00163-7) DOI

Butovskaya M, Rostovstseva V, Mezentseva A, Kavina A, Rizwan M, Shi Y, Vilimek V, Davletshin A. 2024. Cross-cultural perception of strength, attractiveness, aggressiveness and helpfulness of Maasai male faces calibrated to handgrip strength. Sci. Rep. 14, 5880. (10.1038/s41598-024-56607-z) PubMed DOI PMC

Coetzee V, Greeff JM, Stephen ID, Perrett DI. 2014. Cross-cultural agreement in facial attractiveness preferences: the role of ethnicity and gender. PLoS One 9, e99629. (10.1371/journal.pone.0099629) PubMed DOI PMC

Jones D, Hill K. 1993. Criteria of facial attractiveness in five populations. Hum. Nat. 4, 271–296. (10.1007/BF02692202) PubMed DOI

Phalane K, Tribe C, Steel H, Cholo M, Coetzee V. 2017. Facial appearance reveals immunity in African men. Sci. Rep. 7, 7443. (10.1038/s41598-017-08015-9) PubMed DOI PMC

Tureček P. 2025. Violence is associated with preference for masculine faces. OSF. See 10.17605/OSF.IO/4K68N. PubMed DOI

Tureček P, Černý V, Diallo MY, Cissé N, Pokorný Š, Kleisner K. 2025. Supplementary material from: Violence exposure is associated with preference for masculine faces: evidence from Senegal. Figshare (10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7684185) PubMed DOI

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

Violence exposure is associated with preference for masculine faces: evidence from Senegal

. 2025 Mar ; 292 (2042) : 20243105. [epub] 20250312

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...