Advertising cooperative phenotype through costly signals facilitates collective action

. 2022 May ; 9 (5) : 202202. [epub] 20220524

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Velká Británie, Anglie Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid35620016

Around the world, people engage in practices that involve self-inflicted pain and apparently wasted resources. Researchers theorized that these practices help stabilize within-group cooperation by assorting individuals committed to collective action. While this proposition was previously studied using existing religious practices, we provide a controlled framework for an experimental investigation of various predictions derived from this theory. We recruited 372 university students in the Czech Republic who were randomly assigned into either a high-cost or low-cost condition and then chose to play a public goods game (PGG) either in a group that wastes money to signal commitment to high contributions in the game or to play in the group without such signals. We predicted that cooperators would assort in the high-cost revealed group and that, despite these costs, they would contribute more to the common pool and earn larger individual rewards over five iterations of PGG compared with the concealed group and participants in the low-cost condition. The results showed that the assortment of cooperators was more effective in the high-cost condition and translated into larger contributions of the remaining endowment to the common pool, but participants in the low-cost revealed group earned the most. We conclude that costly signals can serve as an imperfect assorting mechanism, but the size of the costs needs to be carefully balanced with potential benefits to be profitable.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Peysakhovich A, Nowak MA, Rand DG. 2014. Humans display a ‘cooperative phenotype’ that is domain general and temporally stable. Nat. Commun. 5, 1-8. (10.1038/ncomms5939) PubMed DOI

Zahavi A, Zahavi A. 1999. The handicap principle: a missing piece of Darwin. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Zahavi A. 1975. Mate selection: a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53, 205-214. (10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3) PubMed DOI

Grafen A. 1990. Biological signals as handicaps. J. Theor. Biol. 144, 517-546. (10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8) PubMed DOI

Caro TM. 1994. Ungulate antipredator behaviour: preliminary and comparative data from African Bovids. Behaviour 128, 189-228. (10.1163/156853994x00262) DOI

Petrie M, Halliday T, Sanders C. 1991. Peahens prefer peacocks with elaborate trains. Anim. Behav. 41, 323-331. (10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80484-1) DOI

Krebs JR, Dawkins R. 1984. Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach (eds Krebs J, Davies N), pp. 380-402. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

Maynard Smith J, Harper D. 2003. Animal signals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Searcy W, Nowicki S. 2005. The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Penn DJ, Számadó S. 2019. The handicap principle: how an erroneous hypothesis became a scientific principle. Biol. Rev. 95, 267-290. (10.1111/brv.12563) PubMed DOI PMC

Bergstrom CT, Számadó S, Lachmann M. 2002. Separating equilibria in continuous signalling games. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 357, 1595-1606. (10.1098/rstb.2002.1068) PubMed DOI PMC

Maynard Smith J. 1991. Honest signalling: the Philip Sidney game. Anim. Behav. 42, 1034-1035. (10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80161-7) DOI

Smith EA, Bliege Bird R, Bird DW. 2003. The benefits of costly signaling: Meriam turtle hunters. Behav. Ecol. 14, 116-126. (10.1093/beheco/14.1.116) DOI

Bliege Bird R, Power EA. 2015. Prosocial signaling and cooperation among Martu hunters. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 389-397. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.003) DOI

Lyle H, Smith E, Sullivan R. 2009. Blood donations as costly signals of donor quality. J . Evol. Psychol. 7, 263-286. (10.1556/JEP.7.2009.4.1) DOI

Smith EA. 2004. Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Hum. Nat. 15, 343-364. (10.1007/s12110-004-1013-9) PubMed DOI

Barker JL, Power EA, Heap S, Puurtinen M, Sosis R. 2019. Content, cost, and context: a framework for understanding human signaling systems. Evol. Anthropol. 28, 86-99. (10.1002/evan.21768) PubMed DOI

Cronk L. 2005. The application of animal signaling theory to human phenomena: some thoughts and clarifications. Soc. Sci. Inf. 44, 603-620. (10.1177/0539018405058203) DOI

Bliege Bird R, Smith EA, Bird DW. 2001. The hunting handicap: costly signaling in human foraging strategies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50, 9-19. (10.1007/s002650100338) DOI

Sosis R. 2003. Why aren't we all hutterites? Costly signaling theory and religious behavior. Hum. Nat. 14, 91-127. (10.1007/s12110-003-1000-6) PubMed DOI

Bulbulia J. 2004. The cognitive and evolutionary psychology of religion. Biol. Philos. 19, 655-686. (10.1007/s10539-005-5568-6) DOI

Irons W. 2001. Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In Evolution and the capacity for commitment (ed. Nesse R), pp. 292-302. New York, NY: The Russell Sage Foundation.

Basedow H. 1927. Subincision and kindred rites of the Australian aboriginal. J. R. Anthropol. Inst. Gt. Britain Irel. 57, 123-156. (10.2307/2843680) DOI

Willsersle R. 2009. The optimal sacrifice: a study of voluntary death among the Siberian chukchi. Am. Ethnol. 36, 693-704. (10.1111/j.1548-1425.2009.01204.x) DOI

Turner V. 1969. The ritual process: structure and anti-structure. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Bliege Bird R, Ready E, Power EA. 2018. The social significance of subtle signals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 452-457. (10.1038/s41562-018-0298-3) PubMed DOI

Xygalatas D, Khan S, Lang M, Kundt R, Kundtová Klocová E, Krátký J, Shaver JH. 2019. Effects of extreme ritual practices on psychophysiological well-being. Curr. Anthropol. 60, 699-707. (10.1086/705665) DOI

Purzycki BG, Apicella C, Atkinson QD, Cohen E, McNamara RA, Willard AK, Xygalatas D, Norenzayan A, Henrich J. 2016. Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature 530, 327-330. (10.1038/nature16980) PubMed DOI

Lang M, et al. . 2019. Moralizing gods, impartiality, and religious parochialism across 15 societies. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20190202. (10.1098/rspb.2019.0202) PubMed DOI PMC

Lang M. 2019. The evolutionary paths to collective rituals: an interdisciplinary perspective on the origins and functions of the basic social act. Arch. Psychol. Relig. 41, 224-252. (10.1177/0084672419894682) DOI

Rappaport R. 1999. Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Rappaport R. 2000. Pigs for the ancestors: ritual in the ecology of a New Guinea people. Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.

Ruffle BJ, Sosis R. 2007. Does it pay to pray? Costly ritual and cooperation. BE J. Econ. Anal. Policy 7, 1-35. (10.2202/1935-1682.1629) DOI

Sosis R, Ruffle BJ. 2003. Religious ritual and cooperation: testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 713-722. (10.1086/379260) DOI

Soler M. 2012. Costly signaling, ritual and cooperation: evidence from Candomblé, an Afro-Brazilian religion. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 346-356. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.004) DOI

Power EA. 2017. Social support networks and religiosity in rural South India. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 1-6. (10.1038/s41562-017-0057) DOI

Xygalatas D, Mitkidis P, Fischer R, Reddish P, Skewes J, Geertz AW, Roepstorff A, Bulbulia J. 2013. Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1602-1605. (10.1177/0956797612472910) PubMed DOI

Power EA. 2016. Discerning devotion: testing the signaling theory of religion. Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 82-91. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.003) DOI

Purzycki BG, Arakchaa T. 2013. Ritual behavior and trust in the Tyva republic. Curr. Anthropol. 54, 381-388. (10.1086/670526) DOI

Tan JHW, Vogel C. 2008. Religion and trust: an experimental study. J. Econ. Psychol. 29, 839-848.

Hall DL, Cohen AB, Meyer KK, Varley AH, Brewer GA. 2015. Costly signaling increases trust, even across religious affiliations. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1368-1376. (10.1177/0956797615576473) PubMed DOI

Henrich J, Bauer M, Cassar A, Chytilová J, Purzycki B. 2019. War increases religiosity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 129-135. (10.1038/s41562-018-0512-3) PubMed DOI

Lang M, Krátký J, Xygalatas D. 2020. The role of ritual behavior in anxiety reduction: an investigation of Marathi religious practices in Mauritius. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 375, 20190431. (10.1098/rstb.2019.0431) PubMed DOI PMC

Lang M, Krátký J, Shaver JH, Jerotijević D, Xygalatas D. 2015. Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior. Curr. Biol. 25, 1892-1897. (10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.049) PubMed DOI

Tewari S, Khan S, Hopkins N, Srinivasan N, Reicher S. 2012. Participation in mass gatherings can benefit well-being: longitudinal and control data from a North Indian Hindu pilgrimage event. PLoS ONE 7, 3-7. (10.1371/journal.pone.0047291) PubMed DOI PMC

Nichols AD, Lang M, Kavanagh C, Kundt R, Yamada J, Ariely D, Mitkidis P. 2020. Replicating and extending the effects of auditory religious cues on dishonest behavior. PLoS ONE 15, e0237007. (10.1371/journal.pone.0237007) PubMed DOI PMC

Lang M, Mitkidis P, Kundt R, Nichols A, Krajčíková L, Xygalatas D. 2016. Music as a sacred cue? Effects of religious music on moral behavior. Front. Psychol. 7, 1-13. (10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00814) PubMed DOI PMC

Aimone JA, Iannaccone LR, Makowsky MD, Rubin J. 2013. Endogenous group formation via unproductive costs. Rev. Econ. Stud. 80, 1215-1236. (10.1093/restud/rdt017) PubMed DOI PMC

van Vugt M, Hardy CL.. 2010. Cooperation for reputation: wasteful contributions as costly signals in public goods. Gr. Process. Intergr. Relations 13, 101-111. (10.1177/1368430209342258) DOI

Roberts G. 2020. Honest signaling of cooperative intentions. Behav. Ecol. 31, 922-932. (10.1093/beheco/araa035) DOI

Padoa-Schioppa C. 2011. Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 333-359. (10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113648) PubMed DOI PMC

Fischbacher U, Gächter S, Fehr E. 2001. Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Econ. Lett. 71, 397-404. (10.1016/S0165-1765(01)00394-9) DOI

Bowles S. 2006. Group competition, reproductive leveling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science 314, 1569-1572. (10.1126/science.1134829) PubMed DOI

Sosis R, Kress HC, Boster JS. 2007. Scars for war: Evaluating alternative signaling explanations for cross-cultural variance in ritual costs. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 234-247. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.02.007) DOI

Soler M, Batiste F, Cronk L. 2014. In the eye (and ears) of the beholder: receiver psychology and human signal design. Evol. Anthropol. 23, 136-145. (10.1002/evan.21413) PubMed DOI

Burton-Chellew MN, El Mouden C, West SA. 2017. Evidence for strategic cooperation in humans. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20170689. (10.1098/rspb.2017.0689) PubMed DOI PMC

Milinski M, Semmann D, Krambeck HJ. 2002. Reputation helps solve the ‘tragedy of the commons'. Nature 415, 424-426. (10.1038/415424a) PubMed DOI

Green P, Macleod CJ. 2016. SIMR: an R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 493-498. (10.1111/2041-210X.12504) DOI

Core Team R. 2020. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Grün B, Leisch F. 2008. Flexmix version 2: finite mixtures with concomitant variables and varying and constant parameters. J. Stat. Softw. 28, 1-35. (10.18637/jss.v028.i04) PubMed DOI

Chen F, Fischbacher U. 2020. Cognitive processes underlying distributional preferences: a response time study. Exp. Econ. 23, 421-446. (10.1007/s10683-019-09618-x) DOI

Ferrari S, Cribari-Neto F. 2004. Beta regression for modelling rates and proportions. J. Appl. Stat. 31, 799-815. (10.1080/0266476042000214501) DOI

Cribari-Neto F, Zeileis A.2009. Beta regression in R. See https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/betareg/vignettes/betareg.pdf .

Stasinopoulos DM, Rigby RA. 2007. Generalized additive models for location scale and shape (GAMLSS) in R. J. Stat. Softw. 23, 1-46. (10.18637/jss.v023.i07) DOI

Reigstad AG, Strømland EA, Tinghög G. 2017. Extending the cooperative phenotype: assessing the stability of cooperation across countries. Front. Psychol. 8, 1990. (10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01990) PubMed DOI PMC

Chaudhuri A. 2011. Sustaining cooperation in laboratory public goods experiments: a selective survey of the literature. Exp. Econ. 14, 47-83. (10.1007/s10683-010-9257-1) DOI

Rigby R, Stasinopoulos M, Heller G, De Bastiani F. 2019. Distributions for modelling location, scale and shape: using GAMLSS in R. New York, NY: CRC Press. See http://www.gamlss.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/01/DistributionsForModellingLocationScaleandShape.pdf.

Chvaja R, Chinchilla J, Gomez A, Lang M. In press. Religious costly signals are more efficient in inducing trustworthiness than secular costly signals.

Lang M, Chvaja R, Grant Purzycki B, Václavík D, Staněk R. 2022. Advertising cooperative phenotype through costly signals facilitates collective action. Figshare. (10.6084/m9.figshare.c.5986008) PubMed DOI PMC

Zobrazit více v PubMed

figshare
10.6084/m9.figshare.c.5986008

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

    Možnosti archivace