Moralizing gods, impartiality and religious parochialism across 15 societies

. 2019 Mar 13 ; 286 (1898) : 20190202.

Jazyk angličtina Země Velká Británie, Anglie Médium print

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články, práce podpořená grantem

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid30836871

The emergence of large-scale cooperation during the Holocene remains a central problem in the evolutionary literature. One hypothesis points to culturally evolved beliefs in punishing, interventionist gods that facilitate the extension of cooperative behaviour toward geographically distant co-religionists. Furthermore, another hypothesis points to such mechanisms being constrained to the religious ingroup, possibly at the expense of religious outgroups. To test these hypotheses, we administered two behavioural experiments and a set of interviews to a sample of 2228 participants from 15 diverse populations. These populations included foragers, pastoralists, horticulturalists, and wage labourers, practicing Buddhism, Christianity, and Hinduism, but also forms of animism and ancestor worship. Using the Random Allocation Game (RAG) and the Dictator Game (DG) in which individuals allocated money between themselves, local and geographically distant co-religionists, and religious outgroups, we found that higher ratings of gods as monitoring and punishing predicted decreased local favouritism (RAGs) and increased resource-sharing with distant co-religionists (DGs). The effects of punishing and monitoring gods on outgroup allocations revealed between-site variability, suggesting that in the absence of intergroup hostility, moralizing gods may be implicated in cooperative behaviour toward outgroups. These results provide support for the hypothesis that beliefs in monitoring and punitive gods help expand the circle of sustainable social interaction, and open questions about the treatment of religious outgroups.

Centre for Culture and Evolution Brunel University London Middlesex UB8 3PH UK

Department of Anthropology Ball State University Muncie IN 47306 USA

Department of Anthropology Montclair State University Montclair NJ 07043 USA

Department of Anthropology University of California Davis Davis CA 95616 USA

Department of Anthropology University of Connecticut Storrs CT 06269 USA

Department of Anthropology Washington University in St Louis St Louis MO 63130 USA

Department of Economics and Government Harvard University Cambridge MA 02138 USA

Department of Human Behavior Ecology and Culture Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig 04103 Germany

Department of Human Evolutionary Biology Harvard University Cambridge MA 02138 USA

Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History Jena 07745 Germany

Department of Psychology University of Auckland Auckland New Zealand

Department of Psychology University of British Columbia Vancouver BC Canada V6T 1Z4

Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia PA 6241 USA

Institute of Human Origins Arizona State University Tempe AZ 4101 USA

LEVYNA Laboratory for the Experimental Research of Religion Masaryk University Brno 602 00 Czech Republic

School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography University of Oxford Oxford OX2 6PE UK

School of Psychology Victoria University of Wellington Wellington 6140 New Zealand

Social Science Sub Division College of DuPage Glen Ellyn IL 60137 USA

Wadham College University of Oxford Oxford OX2 6PE UK

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Henrich J. 2015. The secret of our success: how culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Turchin P. 2016. Ultrasociety: how 10,000 years of war made humans the greatest cooperators on earth. Chaplin, CT: Berresta Books.

Boyd R. 2017. A different kind of animal: how culture transformed Our species. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Henrich J, Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 2012. The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 657–669. (10.1098/rstb.2011.0290) PubMed DOI PMC

Richerson P, et al. 2016. Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: a sketch of the evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 39, e30 (10.1017/S0140525X1400106X) PubMed DOI

Henrich J, Ensminger J, Mcelreath R, Barr A, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E. 2011. Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327, 1480–1484. (10.1126/science.1182238) PubMed DOI

Norenzayan A. 2013. Big gods: how religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Purzycki BG, et al. 2018. The evolution of religion and morality: a synthesis of ethnographic and experimental evidence from eight societies. Relig. Brain Behav. 8, 101–132. (10.1080/2153599X.2016.1267027) DOI

Bowles S. 2011. Cultivation of cereals by the first farmers was not more productive than foraging. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 4760 (10.1073/pnas.1010733108) PubMed DOI PMC

Johnson AW, Earle TK. 2000. The evolution of human societies: from foraging group to agrarian state. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Marcus J, Flannery KV. 2004. The coevolution of ritual and society: new 14C dates from ancient Mexico. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 18 257–18 261. (10.1073/pnas.0408551102) PubMed DOI PMC

Whitehouse H, Hodder I. 2010. Modes of religiosity at Catalhoyuk. In Religion in the emergency of civilization: Çatalhöyük as a case study (ed. Hodder I.), pp. 122–145. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Watts J, Greenhill SJ, Atkinson QD, Currie TE, Bulbulia J, Gray RD. 2015. Broad supernatural punishment but not moralizing high gods precede the evolution of political complexity in Austronesia. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 1–7. (10.1098/rspb.2014.2556) PubMed DOI PMC

Botero CA, Gardner B, Kirby KR, Bulbulia J, Gavin MC, Gray RD. 2014. The ecology of religious beliefs. PNAS 111, 16 784–16 789. (10.1073/pnas.1408701111) PubMed DOI PMC

Johnson D. 2005. God's punishment and public goods. Hum. Nat. 16, 410–446. (10.1007/s12110-005-1017-0) PubMed DOI

Peoples HC, Marlowe FW. 2012. Subsistence and the evolution of religion. Hum. Nat. 23, 253–269. (10.1007/s12110-012-9148-6) PubMed DOI

Atkinson QD, Bourrat P. 2011. Beliefs about God, the afterlife and morality support the role of supernatural policing in human cooperation. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 41–49. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.07.008) DOI

Lang M, Mitkidis P, Kundt R, Nichols A, Krajčíková L, Xygalatas D. 2016. Music as a sacred cue? Effects of religious music on moral behavior. Front. Psychol. 7, 1–13. (10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00814) PubMed DOI PMC

Tan J, Vogel C. 2008. Religion and trust: an experimental study. J. Econ. Psychol. 29, 839–848. (10.1016/j.joep.2008.03.002) DOI

Preston JL, Ritter RS. 2013. Different effects of religion and God on prosociality with the ingroup and outgroup. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 1471–1483. (10.1177/0146167213499937) PubMed DOI

Shariff AF, Willard AK, Andersen T, Norenzayan A. 2016. Religious priming: a meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20, 27–48. (10.1177/1088868314568811) PubMed DOI

Purzycki BG, Apicella C, Atkinson QD, Cohen E, McNamara RA, Willard AK, Xygalatas D, Norenzayan A, Henrich J. 2016. Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature 530, 327–330. (10.1038/nature16980) PubMed DOI

Norenzayan A, Shariff AF, Gervais WM, Willard AK, McNamara RA, Slingerland E, Henrich J. 2016. The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behav. Brain Sci. 39, 1–65. (10.1017/S0140525X14001356) PubMed DOI

Atran S, Ginges J. 2012. Religious and sacred imperatives in human conflict. Science 336, 855–857. (10.1126/science.1216902) PubMed DOI

Ginges J, Hansen I, Norenzayan A. 2009. Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychol. Sci. 20, 224–230. (10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02270.x) PubMed DOI

Choi JK, Bowles S. 2007. The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318, 636–640. (10.1126/science.1144237) PubMed DOI

Fessler DMT, et al. . 2015. Moral parochialism and contextual contingency across seven societies. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20150907 (10.1098/rspb.2015.0907) PubMed DOI PMC

Stark R. 1997. The rise of Christianity: how the obscure, marginal Jesus movement became the dominant religious force in the Western world in a few centuries. San Francisco, CA: Harper Collins Publishers.

Ginges J, Sheikh H, Atran S, Argo N. 2016. Thinking from God's perspective decreases biased valuation of the life of a nonbeliever. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 316–319. (10.1073/pnas.1512120113) PubMed DOI PMC

Clingingsmith D, Khwaja AI, Kremer M. 2009. Estimating the impact of the Hajj: religion and tolerance in Islam's global gathering. Q. J. Econ. 124, 1133–1170. (10.1162/qjec.2009.124.3.1133) DOI

McCullough ME, Swartwout P, Shaver JH, Carter EC, Sosis R. 2016. Christian religious badges instill trust in Christian and non-Christian perceivers. Psycholog. Relig. Spiritual. 8, 149–163. (10.1037/rel0000045) DOI

Bushman BJ, Ridge RD, Das E, Key CW, Busath GL. 2007. When God sanctions killing: effect of scriptural violence on aggression. Psychol. Sci. 18, 204–207. (10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01873.x) PubMed DOI

Shaver JH, Lang M, Krátký J, Klocová EK, Kundt R, Xygalatas D. 2018. The boundaries of trust: cross-religious and cross-ethnic field experiments in Mauritius. Evol. Psychol. 16, 1–15. (10.1177/1474704918817644) PubMed DOI PMC

Purzycki BG. 2016. The evolution of Gods’ minds in the Tyva Republic. Curr. Anthropol. 57, S88–S104. (10.1086/685729) DOI

Purzycki BG, et al. . 2018. The cognitive and cultural foundations of moral behavior. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 490–501. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.04.004). DOI

Hruschka D, et al. . 2014. Impartial institutions, pathogen stress and the expanding social network. Hum. Nat. 25, 567–579. (10.1007/s12110-014-9217-0) PubMed DOI

Engel C. 2011. Dictator games: a meta study. Exp. Econ. 14, 583–610. (10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7) DOI

Ensminger J, Henrich J. 2014. Experimenting with social norms: fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective. New York: NY: Russell Sage Press.

Falk A, Becker A, Dohmen T, Enke B, Huffman D, Sunde U. 2018. Global evidence on economic preferences. Q. J. Economics 133, 1645–1692. (10.1093/qje/qjy013) DOI

Kröll M, Rustagi D. 2016. Shades of dishonesty and cheating in informal milk markets in India. SAFE Work. Pap. No. 134.

Gächter S, Schulz JF. 2016. Intrinsic honesty and the prevalence of rule violations across societies. Nature 531, 1–11. (10.1038/nature17160) PubMed DOI PMC

Purzycki BG. 2011. Tyvan cher eezi and the socioecological constraints of supernatural agents’ minds. Relig. Brain Behav. 1, 31–45. (10.1080/2153599X.2010.550723) DOI

McNamara RA, Henrich J. 2018. Jesus vs. the ancestors: how specific religious beliefs shape prosociality on Yasawa Island, Fiji. Religion. Brain Behav. 8, 185–204. (10.1080/2153599X.2016.1267030) DOI

Johnson KA, Cohen AB, Okun MA. 2016. God is watching you…but also watching over you: the influence of benevolent God representations on secular volunteerism among Christians. Psycholog. Relig. Spiritual. 8, 363–374. (10.1037/rel0000040) DOI

Norenzayan A. 2013. Big gods: how religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Yilmaz O, Bahçekapili HG. 2016. Supernatural and secular monitors promote human cooperation only if they remind of punishment. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 79–84. (10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.09.005) DOI

Purzycki BG, Lang M. 2019. Identity fusion, outgroup relations, and sacrifice: a cross-cultural test. Cognition 186, 1–6. (10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.015) PubMed DOI

Henrich N, Henrich J. 2007. Why humans cooperate: a cultural and evolutionary explanation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

figshare
10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4413395

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...