Nocturnal exposure to a preferred ambient scent does not affect dream emotionality or post-sleep core affect valence in young adults

. 2024 May 06 ; 14 (1) : 10369. [epub] 20240506

Jazyk angličtina Země Velká Británie, Anglie Médium electronic

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články, práce podpořená grantem

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid38710748

Grantová podpora
Cooperatio Program, research area PSYC Ministerstvo Školství, Mládeže a Tělovýchovy

Odkazy

PubMed 38710748
PubMed Central PMC11074142
DOI 10.1038/s41598-024-60226-z
PII: 10.1038/s41598-024-60226-z
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje

Emotions experienced within sleep mentation (dreaming) affect mental functioning in waking life. There have been attempts at enhancing dream emotions using olfactory stimulation. Odors readily acquire affective value, but to profoundly influence emotional processing, they should bear personal significance for the perceiver rather than be generally pleasant. The main objective of the present sleep laboratory study was to examine whether prolonged nocturnal exposure to self-selected, preferred ambient room odor while asleep influences emotional aspects of sleep mentation and valence of post-sleep core affect. We asked twenty healthy participants (12 males, mean age 25 ± 4 years) to pick a commercially available scented room diffuser cartridge that most readily evoked positively valenced mental associations. In weekly intervals, the participants attended three sessions. After the adaptation visit, they were administered the odor exposure and odorless control condition in a balanced order. Participants were awakened five minutes into the first rapid eye movement (REM) stage that took place after 2:30 a.m. and, if they had been dreaming, they were asked to rate their mental sleep experience for pleasantness, emotional charge, and magnitude of positive and negative emotions and also to evaluate their post-sleep core affect valence. With rs < 0.20, no practically or statistically significant differences existed between exposure and control in any outcome measures. We conclude that in young, healthy participants, the practical value of olfactory stimulation with self-selected preferred scents for enhancement of dream emotions and post-sleep core affect valence is very limited.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Fagioli I. Mental activity during sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 2002;6(4):307–320. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0214. PubMed DOI

Yu CK-C. Emotions before, during, and after dreaming sleep. Dreaming. 2007;17(2):73–86. doi: 10.1037/1053-0797.17.2.73. DOI

Wasserman I, Ballif BL. Perceived interactions between the dream and the waking divisions of consciousness. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 1984;4(1):3–13. doi: 10.2190/3N4G-FFRM-25L9-HCXW. DOI

Schredl M, Doll E. Emotions in diary dreams. Conscious. Cogn. 1998;7(4):634–646. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1998.0356. PubMed DOI

Nielsen TA, Deslauriers D, Baylor GW. Emotions in dream and waking event reports. Dreaming. 1991;1(4):287–300. doi: 10.1037/h0094340. DOI

Merritt JM, et al. Emotion profiles in the dreams of men and women. Conscious. Cogn. Int. J. 1994;3(1):46–60. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1994.1004. DOI

Zadra A, Pilon M, Donderi DC. Variety and intensity of emotions in nightmares and bad dreams. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2006;194(4):249–254. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000207359.46223.dc. PubMed DOI

American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™. 5. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; 2013. p. 947.

Simor P, et al. Disturbed dreaming and sleep quality: Altered sleep architecture in subjects with frequent nightmares. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2012;262(8):687–696. doi: 10.1007/s00406-012-0318-7. PubMed DOI

Paul F, Schredl M, Alpers GW. Nightmares affect the experience of sleep quality but not sleep architecture: An ambulatory polysomnographic study. Borderline Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysregul. 2015;2:3. doi: 10.1186/s40479-014-0023-4. PubMed DOI PMC

Kelly WE. Bad dreams and bad sleep: Relationships between nightmare frequency, insomnia, and nightmare proneness. Dreaming. 2022;32(2):194–205. doi: 10.1037/drm0000203. DOI

Blagrove M, Farmer L, Williams E. The relationship of nightmare frequency and nightmare distress to well-being. J. Sleep Res. 2004;13(2):129–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00394.x. PubMed DOI

Schredl M. The effect of dreams on waking life. Sleep Hypn. 2000;2(3):120–124.

Schredl M, Reinhard I. The continuity between waking mood and dream emotions: Direct and second-order effects. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 2010;29(3):271–282. doi: 10.2190/IC.29.3.f. DOI

Sikka P, et al. Negative dream affect is associated with next-day affect level, but not with affect reactivity or affect regulation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2022;16:981289. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.981289. PubMed DOI PMC

Olsen MR, Schredl M, Carlsson I. Conscious use of dreams in waking life (nontherapy setting) for decision-making, problem-solving, attitude formation, and behavioral change. Dreaming. 2020;30(3):257–266. doi: 10.1037/drm0000138. DOI

Schredl M. Effect of dreams on daytime mood: The effects of gender and personality. Sleep Hypn. 2009;11(2):51–57.

De Koninck J, Brunette R. Presleep suggestion related to a phobic object: Successful manipulation of reported dream affect. J. Gen. Psychol. 1991;118(3):185–200. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1991.9917780. PubMed DOI

Cartwright RD, et al. Effect of an erotic movie on the sleep and dreams of young men. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1969;20(3):262–271. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1969.01740150006002. PubMed DOI

Goodenough DR, et al. The effects of stress films on dream affect and on respiration and eye-movement activity during Rapid-Eye-Movement sleep. Psychophysiology. 1975;12(3):313–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01298.x. PubMed DOI

Carpenter KA. The effects of positive and negative pre-sleep stimuli on dream experiences. J. Psychol. 1988;122(1):33–37. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1988.10542940. PubMed DOI

Kontaris I, East BS, Wilson DA. Behavioral and neurobiological convergence of odor, mood and emotion: A review. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2020;14:35. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00035. PubMed DOI PMC

Kadohisa M. Effects of odor on emotion, with implications. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013;7:66. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00066. PubMed DOI PMC

Khan RM, et al. Predicting odor pleasantness from odorant structure: Pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world. J. Neurosci. 2007;27(37):10015–10023. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1158-07.2007. PubMed DOI PMC

Lundstrom JN, Olsson MJ. Subthreshold amounts of social odorant affect mood, but not behavior, in heterosexual women when tested by a male, but not a female, experimenter. Biol. Psychol. 2005;70(3):197–204. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.01.008. PubMed DOI

Lundström JN, et al. Psychological effects of subthreshold exposure to the putative human pheromone 4,16-androstadien-3-one. Horm. Behav. 2003;44(5):395–401. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.06.004. PubMed DOI

Soudry Y, et al. Olfactory system and emotion: Common substrates. Eur. Ann. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Dis. 2011;128(1):18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.anorl.2010.09.007. PubMed DOI

Phelps EA. Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2004;14(2):198–202. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015. PubMed DOI

Phelps EA, LeDoux JE. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron. 2005;48(2):175–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025. PubMed DOI

Zald DH, Pardo JV. Functional neuroimaging of the olfactory system in humans. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2000;36(2):165–181. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00110-5. PubMed DOI

Savic I, et al. Olfactory functions are mediated by parallel and hierarchical processing. Neuron. 2000;26(3):735–745. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81209-X. PubMed DOI

Phan KL, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;16(2):331–348. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. PubMed DOI

De Houwer J, Baeyens F, Field AP. Associative learning of likes and dislikes: Some current controversies and possible ways forward. Cogn. Emot. 2005;19(2):161–174. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000265. PubMed DOI

Levey AB, Martin I. Evaluative conditioning: Overview and further options. Cogn. Emot. 1990;4(1):31–37. doi: 10.1080/02699939008406762. DOI

Zucco GM, Paolini M, Schaal B. Unconscious odour conditioning 25 years later: Revisiting and extending ‘Kirk-Smith, Van Toller and Dodd’. Learn. Motiv. 2009;40(4):364–375. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2009.05.001. DOI

Baeyens F, et al. Toilet rooms, body massages, and smells: Two field studies on human evaluative odor conditioning. Curr. Psychol. 1996;15(1):77–96. doi: 10.1007/BF02686936. DOI

Robin O, et al. Emotional responses evoked by dental odors: An evaluation from autonomic parameters. J. Dent. Res. 1998;77(8):1638–1646. doi: 10.1177/00220345980770081201. PubMed DOI

Robin O, et al. Basic emotions evoked by eugenol odor differ according to the dental experience. A neurovegetative analysis. Chem. Senses. 1999;24(3):327–335. doi: 10.1093/chemse/24.3.327. PubMed DOI

Herz RS, et al. Influences of odors on mood and affective cognition. In: Laing DG, et al., editors. Olfaction, Taste, and Cognition. Cambridge University Press; 2002. pp. 160–177.

Seubert J, et al. Mood Induction with olfactory stimuli reveals differential affective responses in males and females. Chem. Senses. 2009;34(1):77–84. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn054. PubMed DOI

Weber ST, Heuberger E. The impact of natural odors on affective states in humans. Chem. Senses. 2008;33(5):441–447. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn011. PubMed DOI

Alaoui-Ismaïli O, et al. Basic emotions evoked by odorants: Comparison between autonomic responses and self-evaluation. Physiol. Behav. 1997;62(4):713–720. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(97)90016-0. PubMed DOI

Alaoui-Ismaïli O, et al. Odor hedonics: Connection with emotional response estimated by autonomic parameters. Chem. Senses. 1997;22(3):237–248. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.3.237. PubMed DOI

Vernet-Maury E, et al. Basic emotions induced by odorants: A new approach based on autonomic pattern results. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 1999;75(2–3):176–183. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1838(98)00168-4. PubMed DOI

Billot PE, et al. Cerebral bases of emotion regulation toward odours: A first approach. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;317:37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.027. PubMed DOI

Okabe S, et al. Favorite odor induces negative dream emotion during rapid eye movement sleep. Sleep Med. 2018;47:72–76. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.03.026. PubMed DOI

Schredl M, et al. Information processing during sleep: The effect of olfactory stimuli on dream content and dream emotions. J. Sleep Res. 2009;18(3):285–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00737.x. PubMed DOI

Vitinius F, et al. Feasibility of an interval, inspiration-triggered nocturnal odorant application by a novel device: A patient-blinded, randomised crossover, pilot trial on mood and sleep quality of depressed female inpatients. Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 2014;271(9):2443–2454. doi: 10.1007/s00405-013-2873-6. PubMed DOI

Sabiniewicz A, et al. Effects of odors on sleep quality in 139 healthy participants. Sci. Rep. 2022;12(1):17165. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-21371-5. PubMed DOI PMC

Rouby C, Pouliot S, Bensafi M. Odor hedonics and their modulators. Food Qual. Prefer. 2009;20(8):545–549. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.05.004. DOI

Okabe S, et al. Presentation of familiar odor induces negative dream emotions during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in healthy adolescents. Sleep Med. 2020;66:227–232. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.11.1260. PubMed DOI

Okabe S, Abe T. Subjectively intense odor does not affect dream emotions during rapid eye movement sleep. Sci. Rep. 2023;13(1):10442. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-37151-8. PubMed DOI PMC

Villemure C, Slotnick BM, Bushnell MC. Effects of odors on pain perception: Deciphering the roles of emotion and attention. Pain. 2003;106(1–2):101–108. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00297-5. PubMed DOI

Nielsen TA. A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: "Covert" REM sleep as a possible reconciliation of two opposing models. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000;23(6):851–866. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0000399X. PubMed DOI

McNamara P, et al. REM and NREM sleep mentation. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2010;92:69–86. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(10)92004-7. PubMed DOI

Davidson P, et al. Does sleep selectively strengthen certain memories over others based on emotion and perceived future relevance? Nat. Sci. Sleep. 2021;13:1257–1306. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S286701. PubMed DOI PMC

Ferguson CJ. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2009;40(5):532–538. doi: 10.1037/a0015808. DOI

Dalton P, et al. The use of semantic differential scaling to define the multidimensional representation of odors. J. Sens. Stud. 2008;23(4):485–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2008.00167.x. PubMed DOI PMC

Herz RS. Aromatherapy facts and fictions: A scientific analysis of olfactory effects on mood, physiology and behavior. Int. J. Neurosci. 2009;119(2):263–290. doi: 10.1080/00207450802333953. PubMed DOI

Engen T. The acquisition of odour hedonic. In: Van Toller S, Dodd GH, editors. Perfumery: The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance. Chapman & Hall; 1988. pp. 79–90.

Sutton D, et al. Optimizing arousal to manage aggression: A pilot study of sensory modulation. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2013;22(6):500–511. doi: 10.1111/inm.12010. PubMed DOI

Wright L, Bennett S, Meredith P. ‘Why didn't you just give them PRN?’ A qualitative study investigating the factors influencing implementation of sensory modulation approaches in inpatient mental health units. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2020;29(4):608–621. doi: 10.1111/inm.12693. PubMed DOI

Wallis K, Sutton D, Bassett S. Sensory modulation for people with anxiety in a community mental health setting. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health. 2018;34(2):122–137. doi: 10.1080/0164212X.2017.1363681. DOI

Hayes C, et al. What nonpharmacological therapeutic interventions are provided to adolescents admitted to general mental health inpatient units? A descriptive review. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2019;28(3):671–686. doi: 10.1111/inm.12575. PubMed DOI

Keptner KM, Fitzgibbon C, O’Sullivan J. Effectiveness of anxiety reduction interventions on test anxiety: A comparison of four techniques incorporating sensory modulation. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2021;84(5):289–297. doi: 10.1177/0308022620935061. DOI

Todd C, Smothers M, Colson T. Implementing SEL in the classroom: A practitioner perspective. Clear. House J. Educ. Strateg. Issues Ideas. 2022;95(1):18–25. doi: 10.1080/00098655.2021.2016566. DOI

Dorn E, Hitch D, Stevenson C. An evaluation of a sensory room within an adult mental health rehabilitation unit. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health. 2020;36(2):105–118. doi: 10.1080/0164212X.2019.1666770. DOI

Bobier C, et al. Pilot investigation of the use and usefulness of a sensory modulation room in a child and adolescent psychiatric inpatient unit. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health. 2015;31(4):385–401. doi: 10.1080/0164212X.2015.1076367. DOI

Mills CJ, et al. Evaluating a virtual reality sensory room for adults with disabilities. Sci. Rep. 2023;13(1):495. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-26100-6. PubMed DOI PMC

Haig S, Hallett N. Use of sensory rooms in adult psychiatric inpatient settings: A systematic review and narrative synthesis. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 2023;32(1):54–75. doi: 10.1111/inm.13065. PubMed DOI

Cavanagh B, et al. It's like another world: The perceived beneficial effects of an artistically designed multisensory environment. Med. Humanit. 2019;45(1):52–59. doi: 10.1136/medhum-2018-011492. PubMed DOI

Ferdenzi C, et al. Variability of affective responses to odors: Culture, gender, and olfactory knowledge. Chem. Senses. 2013;38(2):175–186. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjs083. PubMed DOI

Ferdenzi C, et al. Individual differences in verbal and non-verbal affective responses to smells: Influence of odor label across cultures. Chem. Senses. 2017;42(1):37–46. PubMed

Arshamian A, et al. The perception of odor pleasantness is shared across cultures. Curr. Biol. 2022;32(9):2061–2066.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.02.062. PubMed DOI PMC

Ayabe-Kanamura S, et al. Differences in perception of everyday odors: A Japanese-German cross-cultural study. Chem. Senses. 1998;23(1):31–38. doi: 10.1093/chemse/23.1.31. PubMed DOI

Kermen F, et al. Molecular complexity determines the number of olfactory notes and the pleasantness of smells. Sci. Rep. 2011;1(1):206. doi: 10.1038/srep00206. PubMed DOI PMC

Keller A, et al. Predicting human olfactory perception from chemical features of odor molecules. Science. 2017;355(6327):820–826. doi: 10.1126/science.aal2014. PubMed DOI PMC

Zarzo M. Hedonic judgments of chemical compounds are correlated with molecular size. Sensors (Basel) 2011;11(4):3667–3686. doi: 10.3390/s110403667. PubMed DOI PMC

Moskowitz HR, Dravnieks A, Klarman LA. Odor intensity and pleasantness for a diverse set of odorants. Percept. Psychophys. 1976;19(2):122–128. doi: 10.3758/BF03204218. DOI

Henion KE. Odor pleasantness and intensity: A single dimension? J. Exp. Psychol. 1971;90(2):275–279. doi: 10.1037/h0031549. PubMed DOI

Doty RL. An examination of relationships between the pleasantness, intensity, and concentration of 10 odorous stimuli. Percept. Psychophys. 1975;17(5):492–496. doi: 10.3758/BF03203300. DOI

Smeets MA, Dijksterhuis GB. Smelly primes: When olfactory primes do or do not work. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:96. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00096. PubMed DOI PMC

Herz RS. The role of odor-evoked memory in psychological and physiological health. Brain Sci. 2016 doi: 10.3390/brainsci6030022. PubMed DOI PMC

Hackländer RPM, Janssen SMJ, Bermeitinger C. An in-depth review of the methods, findings, and theories associated with odor-evoked autobiographical memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2019;26(2):401–429. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1545-3. PubMed DOI

Pellegrino R, et al. Habituation and adaptation to odors in humans. Physiol. Behav. 2017;177:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.04.006. PubMed DOI

Sinding C, et al. New determinants of olfactory habituation. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:41047. doi: 10.1038/srep41047. PubMed DOI PMC

Schredl M, et al. Olfactory stimulation during sleep can reactivate odor-associated images. Chemosens. Percept. 2014;7(3–4):140–146. doi: 10.1007/s12078-014-9173-4. DOI

Moss M, et al. Expectancy and the aroma of Roman chamomile influence mood and cognition in healthy volunteers. Int. J. Aromather. 2006;16(2):63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijat.2006.04.002. DOI

Howard S, Hughes BM. Expectancies, not aroma, explain impact of lavender aromatherapy on psychophysiological indices of relaxation in young healthy women. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2008;13(Pt 4):603–617. doi: 10.1348/135910707X238734. PubMed DOI

Martinec Nováková L, et al. Effects of all-night exposure to ambient odour on dreams and affective state upon waking. Physiol. Behav. 2021;230:113265. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113265. PubMed DOI

Doty RL, et al. Smell identification ability: Changes with age. Science. 1984;226(4681):1441–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.6505700. PubMed DOI

Doty RL, Cameron EL. Sex differences and reproductive hormone influences on human odor perception. Physiol. Behav. 2009;97(2):213–228. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.032. PubMed DOI PMC

Doty RL. Clinical disorders of olfaction. In: Doty RL, editor. Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. Wiley; 2015. pp. 375–401.

Jones S, Benca RM. Psychiatric disorders associated with circadian rhythm disturbances. In: Lee-Chiong T, editor. Sleep: A Comprehensive Handbook. Wiley; 2005. pp. 409–414.

Gourineni R, Zee PC. Neurological and medical disorders associated with circadian rhythm disturbances. In: Lee-Chiong T, editor. Sleep: A Comprehensive Handbook. Wiley; 2005. pp. 401–407.

Neuland C, et al. Health-related and specific olfaction-related quality of life in patients with chronic functional anosmia or severe hyposmia. Laryngoscope. 2011;121(4):867–872. doi: 10.1002/lary.21387. PubMed DOI

Frasnelli J, Hummel T. Olfactory dysfunction and daily life. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 2005;262(3):231–235. doi: 10.1007/s00405-004-0796-y. PubMed DOI

Smarr KL, Keefer AL. Measures of depression and depressive symptoms: Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), and Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) Arthritis Care Res. (Hoboken) 2011;63(Suppl 11):S454–S466. PubMed

Preiss M, Vacíř K. BDI-II. Beckova sebeposuzovací škála pro dospělé. Psychodiagnostika; 1999.

Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation; 1996.

Julian LJ. Measures of anxiety: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety (HADS-A) Arthritis Care Res. (Hoboken) 2011;63(11):S467–S472. PubMed PMC

Heretik A, et al. Restandardizace state-trait anxiety inventory X-2 – úzkostnost jako rys. Českoslov. Psychol. 2009;53(6):587–599.

Husten CG. How should we define light or intermittent smoking? Does it matter? Nicotine Tob. Res. 2009;11(2):111–121. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp010. PubMed DOI PMC

Dufour MC. What is moderate drinking? Defining "drinks" and drinking levels. Alcohol Res. Health. 1999;23(1):5–14. PubMed PMC

Cohen JE. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1988.

Brand G, Schaal B. Olfaction in depressive disorders: Issues and perspectives. L'Encephale. 2017;43(2):176–182. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2016.04.008. PubMed DOI

Croy I, Hummel T. Olfaction as a marker for depression. J. Neurol. 2017;264(4):631–638. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8227-8. PubMed DOI

Riemann D, et al. Sleep, insomnia, and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(1):74–89. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0411-y. PubMed DOI PMC

Dinis J, Bragança M. Quality of sleep and depression in college students: A systematic review. Sleep Sci. 2018;11(4):290–301. doi: 10.5935/1984-0063.20180045. PubMed DOI PMC

Kupfer DJ, Frank E, Ehlers CL. EEG sleep in young depressives: first and second night effects. Biol. Psychiatry. 1989;25(1):87–97. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(89)90150-9. PubMed DOI

Rotenberg VS, et al. First night effect in depression: New data and a new approach. Biol. Psychiatry. 1997;42(4):267–274. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00343-5. PubMed DOI

Agnew HW, Jr, Webb WB, Williams RL. The first night effect: An EEG study of sleep. Psychophysiology. 1966;2(3):263–266. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1966.tb02650.x. PubMed DOI

Mendels J, Hawkins DR. Sleep laboratory adaptation in normal subjects and depressed patients ("first night effect") Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1967;22(6):556–558. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(67)90063-6. PubMed DOI

Tonori H, et al. Anxiety and depressive states in multiple chemical sensitivity. Tohoku J. Exp. Med. 2001;193(2):115–126. doi: 10.1620/tjem.193.115. PubMed DOI

Johnson D, Colman I. The association between multiple chemical sensitivity and mental illness: Evidence from a nationally representative sample of Canadians. J. Psychosom. Res. 2017;99:40–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.06.002. PubMed DOI

Riedel BW, Winfield CF, Lichstein KL. First night effect and reverse first night effect in older adults with primary insomnia: Does anxiety play a role? Sleep Med. 2001;2(2):125–133. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00054-X. PubMed DOI

Reynolds CF, et al. EEG sleep in outpatients with generalized anxiety: A preliminary comparison with depressed outpatients. Psychiatry Res. 1983;8(2):81–89. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(83)90094-X. PubMed DOI

Houghton DC, et al. Odor sensitivity impairment: A behavioral marker of psychological distress? CNS Spectr. 2019;24(4):404–412. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001177. PubMed DOI PMC

Takahashi T, et al. Possible relation between olfaction and anxiety in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2015;69(7):431–438. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12277. PubMed DOI

Fortier-Lebel O, et al. Chemosensation in anxiety: The trigeminal system matters. Chem. Senses. 2023;48:bjad010. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjad010. PubMed DOI PMC

Zucco GM, Doty RL. Multiple chemical sensitivity. Brain Sci. 2022;12(1):46. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010046. PubMed DOI PMC

Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: STAI (Form Y) Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.

Caci H, et al. The Spielberger trait anxiety inventory measures more than anxiety. Eur. Psychiatry. 2003;18(8):394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.05.003. PubMed DOI

Fredrickson BL. Positive emotions. In: Snyder CR, Lopez SJ, editors. Handbook of Positive Psychology. Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 120–134.

Domhoff GW. A new neurocognitive theory of dreams. Dreaming. 2001;11(1):13–33. doi: 10.1023/A:1009464416649. DOI

Västfjäll D, et al. The measurement of core affect: A Swedish self-report measure derived from the affect circumplex. Scand. J. Psychol. 2002;43(1):19–31. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00265. PubMed DOI

Russell JA. Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction. Cogn. Emot. 2009;23(7):1259–1283. doi: 10.1080/02699930902809375. DOI

Scherer KR. Neuroscience projections to current debates in emotion psychology. Cogn. Emot. 1993;7(1):1–41. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409174. DOI

Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 2003;110(1):145–172. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145. PubMed DOI

Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS. The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 2005;17(3):715–734. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050340. PubMed DOI PMC

Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980;39(6):1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714. DOI

Latinjak AT, López-Ros V, Font-Lladó R. A descriptive study of emotion concepts using dimensional models of core affect/Un estudio descriptivo de los conceptos emocionales desde los modelos dimensionales de los estados afectivos. Stud. Psychol. 2015;36(2):413–450. doi: 10.1080/02109395.2015.1026121. DOI

Russell JA, Barrett LF. Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999;76(5):805–819. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805. PubMed DOI

Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N. Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. Russell Sage Foundation; 1999. p. 593.

Stepanski EJ, Wyatt JK. Use of sleep hygiene in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med. Rev. 2003;7(3):215–225. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0246. PubMed DOI

Hilditch CJ, McHill AW. Sleep inertia: Current insights. Nat. Sci. Sleep. 2019;11:155–165. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S188911. PubMed DOI PMC

Trotti LM. Waking up is the hardest thing I do all day: Sleep inertia and sleep drunkenness. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017;35:76–84. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.08.005. PubMed DOI PMC

Kanady JC, Harvey AG. Development and validation of the sleep inertia questionnaire (SIQ) and assessment of sleep inertia in analogue and clinical depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2015;39(5):601–612. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9686-4. PubMed DOI PMC

Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania smell identification test: A standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol. Behav. 1984;32:489–502. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90269-5. PubMed DOI

Doty RL. The Smell Identification Test Administration Manual. Sensonics; 1995.

Herz RS, et al. A three-factor benefits framework for understanding consumer preference for scented household products: Psychological interactions and implications for future development. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2022;7(1):28. doi: 10.1186/s41235-022-00378-6. PubMed DOI PMC

Lawless H, Engen T. Associations to odors: Interference, mnemonics, and verbal labeling. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 1977;3(1):52–59. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.3.1.52. PubMed DOI

Rundo JV, Downey R. Chapter 25: Polysomnography. In: Levin KH, Chauvel P, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2019. pp. 381–392. PubMed

Gazea M, et al. Chapter 17: Functions and circuits of REM sleep. In: Dringenberg HC, et al., editors. Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience. Elsevier; 2019. pp. 249–267.

Berry RB, et al. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Version 2.2. American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2015.

Roth T, Stubbs C, Walsh JK. Ramelteon (TAK-375), a selective MT1/MT2-receptor agonist, reduces latency to persistent sleep in a model of transient insomnia related to a novel sleep environment. Sleep. 2005;28(3):303–307. PubMed

Tamaki M, et al. Examination of the first-night effect during the sleep-onset period. Sleep. 2005;28(2):195–202. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.2.195. PubMed DOI

Tamaki M, Nittono H, Hori T. The first-night effect occurs at the sleep-onset period regardless of the temporal anxiety level in healthy students. Sleep Biol. Rhythms. 2005;3(2):92–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2005.00167.x. DOI

Vlahoyiannis A, et al. A critical review on sleep assessment methodologies in athletic populations: Factors to be considered. Sleep Med. 2020;74:211–223. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.07.029. PubMed DOI

Thoresen CJ, et al. The affective underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration. Psychol. Bull. 2003;129(6):914–945. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.914. PubMed DOI

Kritzler S, Krasko J, Luhmann M. Inside the happy personality: Personality states, situation experience, and state affect mediate the relation between personality and affect. J. Res. Personal. 2020;85:103929. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103929. DOI

Scarpelli S, et al. The functional role of dreaming in emotional processes. Front. Psychol. 2019 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00459. PubMed DOI PMC

Field A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. 4. Sage; 2013.

Rosenthal R. Meta-Analytical Procedures for Social Research. Sage; 1991.

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...