Olfaction-Related Factors Affecting Chemosensory Dream Content in a Sleep Laboratory
Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Language English Country Switzerland Media electronic
Document type Journal Article
Grant support
GA17-14534S
Czech Science Foundation
program UNCE/HUM/025 (204056)
Charles University Research Centre
260 610
Specific Academic Research project (Specifický vysokoškolský výzkum, SVV)
PubMed
34573245
PubMed Central
PMC8465492
DOI
10.3390/brainsci11091225
PII: brainsci11091225
Knihovny.cz E-resources
- Keywords
- chemoreception, dream, expectancy, mental sleep experience, odour awareness, odour identification, rapid eye movement, recall, sleep, smell,
- Publication type
- Journal Article MeSH
Mental activity in sleep often involves visual and auditory content. Chemosensory (olfactory and gustatory) experiences are less common and underexplored. The aim of the study was to identify olfaction-related factors that may affect the occurrence of chemosensory dream content. Specifically, we investigated the effects of all-night exposure to an ambient odour, participants' appraisal of their current olfactory environment, their general propensity to notice odours and act on them (i.e., odour awareness), and their olfactory acuity. Sixty pre-screened healthy young adults underwent olfactory assessment, completed a measure of odour awareness, and spent three nights in weekly intervals in a sleep laboratory. The purpose of the first visit was to adapt to the experimental setting. On the second visit, half of them were exposed to the smell of vanillin or thioglycolic acid and the other half to an odourless control condition. On the third visit, they received control or stimulation in a balanced order. On each visit, data were collected twice: once from the first rapid eye movement (REM) stage that occurred after 3 a.m., and then shortly before getting up, usually from a non-REM stage. Participants were asked to report the presence of sensory dream content and to assess their current olfactory environment. Neither exposure, nor participants' assessments of the ambient odour, or olfactory acuity affected reports of chemosensory dream content but they were more frequent in individuals with greater odour awareness. This finding may have implications for treatment when such experiences become unwanted or bothersome.
3rd Faculty of Medicine Charles University Ruská 87 100 00 Prague 10 Vinohrady Czech Republic
National Institute of Mental Health Topolová 748 250 67 Klecany Czech Republic
See more in PubMed
Kryger M.H., Roth T., Dement W.C. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 5th ed. Saunders/Elsevier; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 2011.
Fagioli I. Mental activity during sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 2002;6:307–320. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0214. PubMed DOI
Aserinsky E., Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science. 1953;118:273–274. doi: 10.1126/science.118.3062.273. PubMed DOI
Cipolli C., Ferrara M., de Gennaro L., Plazzi G. Beyond the neuropsychology of dreaming: Insights into the neural basis of dreaming with new techniques of sleep recording and analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017;35:8–20. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.07.005. PubMed DOI
Hobson J.A., Pace-Schott E.F., Stickgold R. Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000;23:793–842. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00003976. PubMed DOI
Foulkes D. Nonrapid eye movement mentation. Exp. Neurol. 1967;19:28–38. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(67)90154-9. PubMed DOI
Solms M. Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000;23:843–850. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00003988. PubMed DOI
McNamara P., Johnson P., McLaren D., Harris E., Beauharnais C., Auerbach S. REM and NREM sleep mentation. In: Clow A., McNamara P., editors. International Review of Neurobiology. Dreams and Dreaming. Volume 92. Academic Press; New York, NY, USA: 2010. pp. 69–86. PubMed
Foulkes D. Dream reports from different stages of sleep. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1962;65:14–25. doi: 10.1037/h0040431. PubMed DOI
Wittmann L., Palmy C., Schredl M. NREM sleep dream recall, dream report length and cortical activation. Sleep Hypn. 2004;6:43–47.
Kahan T.L., Claudatos S. Phenomenological features of dreams: Results from dream log studies using the Subjective Experiences Rating Scale (SERS) Conscious. Cogn. 2016;41:159–176. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.007. PubMed DOI
Kahan T.L., LaBerge S.P. Dreaming and waking: Similarities and differences revisited. Conscious. Cogn. 2011;20:494–514. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.002. PubMed DOI
Strauch L., Meier B., Foulkes D. In Search of Dreams: Results of Experimental Dream Research. State University of New York Press; Albany, NY, USA: 1996.
Aspy D.J., Delfabbro P., Proeve M. Is dream recall underestimated by retrospective measures and enhanced by keeping a logbook? A review. Conscious. Cogn. 2015;33:364–374. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.005. PubMed DOI
McCormick L., Nielsen T., Ptito M., Hassainia F., Ptito A., Villemure J.G., Vera C., Montplaisir J. REM sleep dream mentation in right hemispherectomized patients. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:695–701. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00113-3. PubMed DOI
Okabe S., Fukuda K., Mochizuki-Kawai H., Yamada K. Favorite odor induces negative dream emotion during rapid eye movement sleep. Sleep Med. 2018;47:72–76. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.03.026. PubMed DOI
Okabe S., Hayashi M., Abe T., Fukuda K. Presentation of familiar odor induces negative dream emotions during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in healthy adolescents. Sleep Med. 2020;66:227–232. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.11.1260. PubMed DOI
Schredl M., Atanasova D., Hoermann K., Maurer J.T., Hummel T., Stuck B.A. Information processing during sleep: The effect of olfactory stimuli on dream content and dream emotions. J. Sleep Res. 2009;18:285–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00737.x. PubMed DOI
Schredl M., Hoffmann L., Sommer J.U., Stuck B.A. Olfactory stimulation during sleep can reactivate odor-associated images. Chemosens. Percept. 2014;7:140–146. doi: 10.1007/s12078-014-9173-4. DOI
Solms M. The Neuropsychology of Dreams: A Clinico-Anatomical Study. Psychology Press; London, UK: 2014. Nine patients with recurring nightmares; pp. 202–209.
Solms M. The Neuropsychology of Dreams: A Clinico-Anatomical Study. Psychology Press; London, UK: 2014. Other abnormalities of dreaming described in the literature; pp. 58–70.
Giani L., Casazza R., Mariani C., Lovati C. Sensory modalities during dreams in migraine: Case-control study using a daily questionnaire. Dreaming. 2017;27:260–268. doi: 10.1037/drm0000056. DOI
Lovati C., DeAngeli F., D’Amico D., Giani L., D’Alessandro C.M., Zardoni M., Scaglione V., Castoldi D., Capiluppi E., Curone M., et al. Is the brain of migraineurs “different” even in dreams? Neurol. Sci. 2014;35:167–169. doi: 10.1007/s10072-014-1762-0. PubMed DOI
Epstein A.W. Effect of certain cerebral hemispheric diseases on dreaming. Biol. Psychiatry. 1979;14:77–93. PubMed
Epstein A.W., Freeman N.R. The uncinate focus and dreaming. Epilepsia. 1981;22:603–605. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1981.tb04132.x. PubMed DOI
Mehta K., Vankar G., Patel V. Validity of the construct of post-traumatic stress disorder in a low-income country—Interview study of women in Gujarat, India. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2005;187:585–586. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.585. PubMed DOI
De Saint-Denys L.H. Dreams and How to Guide Them. Gerald Duckworth; London, UK: 1867.
Schäfer L., Schellong J., Hähner A., Weidner K., Hüttenbrink K.-B., Trautmann S., Hummel T., Croy I. Nocturnal olfactory stimulation for improvement of sleep quality in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: A randomized exploratory intervention trial. J. Trauma. Stress. 2019;32:130–140. doi: 10.1002/jts.22359. PubMed DOI
Ackerley R., Croy I., Olausson H., Badre G. Investigating the putative impact of odors purported to have beneficial effects on sleep: Neural and perceptual processes. Chemosens. Percept. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s12078-019-09269-5. DOI
Cometto-Muñiz J.E., Cain W.S. Olfactory adaptation. In: Doty R.L., editor. Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. 1st ed. Marcel Dekker; New York, NY, USA: 1995. pp. 257–281.
Johnson A.J. Cognitive facilitation following intentional odor exposure. Sensors. 2011;11:5469–5488. doi: 10.3390/s110505469. PubMed DOI PMC
Jellinek J.S. Psychodynamic odor effects and their mechanisms: Failure to identify the mechanism can lead to faulty conclusions in odor studies. Cosmet. Toilet. 1997;112:61–72.
Knasko S.C., Gilbert A.N., Sabini J. Emotional state, physical well-being, and performance in the presence of feigned ambient odor. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1990;20:1345–1357. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb01476.x. DOI
Hudson R., Distel H. The individuality of odor perception. In: Rouby C., Schaal B., Dubois D., Gervais R., Holley A., editors. Olfaction, Taste and Cognition. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2002. pp. 408–420.
De Araujo I.E., Rolls E.T., Velazco M.I., Margot C., Cayeux I. Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron. 2005;46:671–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.021. PubMed DOI
Pacharra M., Schaper M., Kleinbeck S., Blaszkewicz M., van Thriel C. Olfactory acuity and automatic associations to odor words modulate adverse effects of ammonia. Chemosens. Percept. 2016;9:27–36. doi: 10.1007/s12078-016-9202-6. DOI
Österberg K., Ørbæk P., Karlson B., Åkesson B., Bergendorf U. Annoyance and performance during the experimental chemical challenge of subjects with multiple chemical sensitivity. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health. 2003;29:40–50. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.703. PubMed DOI
Österberg K., Persson R., Karlson B., Eek F.C., Ørbæk P. Personality, mental distress, and subjective health complaints among persons with environmental annoyance. Hum. Exp. Toxicol. 2007;26:231–241. doi: 10.1177/0960327107070575. PubMed DOI
Österberg K., Persson R., Karlson B., Orbaek P. Annoyance and performance of three environmentally intolerant groups during experimental challenge with chemical odors. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health. 2004;30:486–496. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.838. PubMed DOI
Stevenson R.J., Case T.I. Olfactory dreams: Phenomenology, relationship to volitional imagery and odor identification. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 2004;24:69–90. doi: 10.2190/R97G-RM98-B58N-DUYA. DOI
Weitz H., Croy I., Seo H.S., Negoias S., Hummel T. Studies on olfactory dreaming. Chemosens. Percept. 2010;3:129–134. doi: 10.1007/s12078-010-9074-0. DOI
Smeets M.A.M., Schifferstein H.N.J., Boelema S.R., Lensvelt-Mulders G. The Odor Awareness Scale: A new scale for measuring positive and negative odor awareness. Chem. Senses. 2008;33:725–734. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn038. PubMed DOI
Arshamian A., Willander J., Larsson M. Olfactory awareness is positively associated to odour memory. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2011;23:220–226. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.483226. DOI
Doty R.L. Clinical disorders of olfaction. In: Doty R.L., editor. Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. 3rd ed. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2015. pp. 375–401.
Jones S., Benca R.M. Psychiatric disorders associated with circadian rhythm disturbances. In: Lee-Chiong T., editor. Sleep: A Comprehensive Handbook. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2006. pp. 409–414.
Gourineni R., Zee P.C. Neurological and medical disorders associated with circadian rhythm disturbances. In: Lee-Chiong T., editor. Sleep: A Comprehensive Handbook. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2006. pp. 401–407.
Neuland C., Bitter T., Marschner H., Gudziol H., Guntinas-Lichius O. Health-related and specific olfaction-related quality of life in patients with chronic functional anosmia or severe hyposmia. Laryngoscope. 2011;121:867–872. doi: 10.1002/lary.21387. PubMed DOI
Frasnelli J., Hummel T. Olfactory dysfunction and daily life. Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 2005;262:231–235. doi: 10.1007/s00405-004-0796-y. PubMed DOI
Preiss M., Vacíř K. BDI-II. Beckova sebeposuzovací škála pro dospělé. Psychodiagnostika; Brno, Czech Republic: 1999.
Husten C.G. How should we define light or intermittent smoking? Does it matter? Nicotine Tob. Res. 2009;11:111–121. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp010. PubMed DOI PMC
Dufour M.C. What is moderate drinking? Defining “drinks” and drinking levels. Alcohol Res. Health. 1999;23:5–14. PubMed PMC
Mattos J.L., Schlosser R.J., Storck K.A., Soler Z.M. Understanding the relationship between olfactory-specific quality of life, objective olfactory loss, and patient factors in chronic rhinosinusitis. Int. Forum Allergy Rhinol. 2017;7:734–740. doi: 10.1002/alr.21940. PubMed DOI PMC
Beck A.T., Steer R.A., Brown G.K. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corp.; San Antonio, TX, USA: 1996.
Smarr K.L., Keefer A.L. Measures of depression and depressive symptoms: Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), and Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) Arthritis Care Res. 2011;63:S454–S466. doi: 10.1002/acr.20556. PubMed DOI
Simopoulos E., Katotomichelakis M., Gouveris H., Tripsianis G., Livaditis M., Danielides V. Olfaction-associated quality of life in chronic rhinosinusitis: Adaptation and validation of an olfaction-specific questionnaire. Laryngoscope. 2012;122:1450–1454. doi: 10.1002/lary.23349. PubMed DOI
Nováková L., Valentova J.V., Havlíček J. Engagement in olfaction-related activities is associated with the ability of odor identification and odor awareness. Chemosens. Percept. 2014;7:56–67. doi: 10.1007/s12078-014-9167-2. DOI
Martinec Nováková L., Fialová J., Havlíček J. Effects of diversity in olfactory environment on children’s sense of smell. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:2937. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20236-0. PubMed DOI PMC
Martinec Nováková L., Miletínová E., Kliková M., Bušková J. Effects of all-night exposure to ambient odour on dreams and affective state upon waking. Physiol. Behav. 2021;230:113265. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113265. PubMed DOI
Doty R.L., Shaman P., Applebaum S.L., Giberson R., Siksorski L., Rosenberg L. Smell identification ability: Changes with age. Science. 1984;226:1441–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.6505700. PubMed DOI
Doty R.L., Shaman P., Dann M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania smell identification test: A standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol. Behav. 1984;32:489–502. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90269-5. PubMed DOI
Doty R.L., McKeown D.A., Lee W.W., Shaman P. A study of the test-retest reliability of ten olfactory tests. Chem. Senses. 1995;20:645–656. doi: 10.1093/chemse/20.6.645. PubMed DOI
Stevens J.C., Cain W.S., Burke R.J. Variability of olfactory thresholds. Chem. Senses. 1988;13:643–653. doi: 10.1093/chemse/13.4.643. DOI
Mazzatenta A., Cellerino A., Origlia N., Barloscio D., Sartucci F., Di Giulio C., Domenici L. Olfactory phenotypic expression unveils human aging. Oncotarget. 2016;7:19193–19200. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.8393. PubMed DOI PMC
Arctander S. Perfume and Flavor Chemicals: Aroma Chemicals. The Author; Montclair, NJ, USA: 1969.
Dravnieks A. Atlas of Odor Character Profiles. American Society for Testing and Materials; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 1985.
Doty R.L., Brugger W.E., Jurs P.C., Orndorff M.A., Snyder P.I., Lowry L.D. Intranasal trigeminal stimulation from odorous volatiles: Psychometric responses from anosmic and normal humans. Physiol. Behav. 1978;20:175–185. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90070-7. PubMed DOI
Billot P.-E., Andrieu P., Biondi A., Vieillard S., Moulin T., Millot J.-L. Cerebral bases of emotion regulation toward odours: A first approach. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;317:37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.027. PubMed DOI
Berry R.B., Brooks R., Gamaldo C.E., Harding S.M., Lloyd R.M., Quan S.F., Troester M.M., Vaughn B.V. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Version 2.4. American Academy of Sleep Medicine; Darien, IL, USA: 2017. PubMed PMC
IBM Corporation . IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, 24. IBM Corp.; Armonk, NY, USA: 2016.
SAS . SAS University Edition. SAS Institute Inc.; Cary, NC, USA: 2017.
Fagerland M.W., Lydersen S., Laake P. The McNemar test for binary matched-pairs data: Mid-p and asymptotic are better than exact conditional. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2013;13:91. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-13-91. PubMed DOI PMC
Dean A.G., Sullivan K.M., Soe M.M. OpenEpi: Open Source Epidemiologic Statistics for Public Health. [(accessed on 3 May 2020)]. Available online: https://www.openepi.com.
Freeman G.H., Halton J.H. Note on an exact treatment of contingency, goodness of fit and other problems of significance. Biometrika. 1951;38:141–149. doi: 10.1093/biomet/38.1-2.141. PubMed DOI
Rosenthal R. Meta-Analytical Procedures for Social Research. Sage; London, UK: 1991.
Liang K.-Y., Zeger S.L. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73:13–22. doi: 10.1093/biomet/73.1.13. DOI
Zeger S.L., Liang K.-Y. Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics. 1986;42:121–130. doi: 10.2307/2531248. PubMed DOI
Ramezani N. Analyzing non-normal binomial and categorical response variables under varying data conditions; Proceedings of the SAS Global Forum Conference; Las Vegas, NV, USA. 18–21 April 2016.
Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. 2nd ed. John Wiley & Sons Inc.; New York, NY, USA: 2002.
Pan W. Akaike’s Information Criterion in Generalized Estimating Equations. Biometrics. 2001;57:120–125. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2001.00120.x. PubMed DOI
Hanley J.A., Negassa A., Forrester J.E. Statistical analysis of correlated data using generalized estimating equations: An orientation. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2003;157:364–375. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf215. PubMed DOI
Zheng B. Summarizing the goodness of fit of generalized linear models for longitudinal data. Stat. Med. 2000;19:1265–1275. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(20000530)19:10<1265::AID-SIM486>3.0.CO;2-U. PubMed DOI
Tan T.K., Kang T., Hogan D. Using GEE to model student’s satisfaction: A SAS macro approach; Proceedings of the SAS Global Forum 2009; Washington, DC, USA. 22–25 March 2009; pp. 1–12.
Firth D. Bias reduction of Maximum Likelihood Estimates. Biometrika. 1993;80:27–38. doi: 10.1093/biomet/80.1.27. DOI
Borenstein M., Hedges L.V., Higgins J.P., Rothstein H.R. Converting among effect sizes. In: Borenstein M., Hedges L.V., Higgins J.P., Rothstein H.R., editors. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; Chichester, UK: 2009.
Ferguson C.J. An effect size primer: A Guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof. Psychol.—Res. Pract. 2009;40:532–538. doi: 10.1037/a0015808. DOI
Lorig T.S., Huffman E., DeMartino A., DeMarco J. The effects of low concentration odors on EEG activity and behavior. J. Psychophysiol. 1991;5:69–77.
Kuroda K., Inoue N., Ito Y., Kubota K., Sugimoto A., Kakuda T., Fushiki T. Sedative effects of the jasmine tea odor and (R)-(-)-linalool, one of its major odor components, on autonomic nerve activity and mood states. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2005;95:107–114. doi: 10.1007/s00421-005-1402-8. PubMed DOI
Howard S., Hughes B.M. Expectancies, not aroma, explain impact of lavender aromatherapy on psychophysiological indices of relaxation in young healthy women. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2008;13:603–617. doi: 10.1348/135910707X238734. PubMed DOI
Pellegrino R., Sinding C., de Wijk R.A., Hummel T. Habituation and adaptation to odors in humans. Physiol. Behav. 2017;177:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.04.006. PubMed DOI
Sinding C., Valadier F., Al-Hassani V., Feron G., Tromelin A., Kontaris I., Hummel T. New determinants of olfactory habituation. Sci. Rep. 2017;7 doi: 10.1038/srep41047. PubMed DOI PMC
Wrzesniewski A., McCauley C., Rozin P. Odor and affect: Individual differences in the impact of odor on liking for places, things and people. Chem. Senses. 1999;24:713–721. doi: 10.1093/chemse/24.6.713. PubMed DOI
Cohen J.E. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; Hillsdale, NJ, USA: 1988.
Homewood J., Stevenson R.J. Differences in naming accuracy of odors presented to the left and right nostrils. Biol. Psychol. 2001;58:65–73. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00097-7. PubMed DOI
Hummel T., Sekinger B., Wolf S.R., Pauli E., Kobal G. ‘Sniffin’ Sticks’: Olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem. Senses. 1997;22:39–52. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.1.39. PubMed DOI
Wayne G.G., Clinco A.A. Psychoanalytic observations on olfaction with special reference to olfactory dreams. Psychoanal. Rev. 1959;46:64–74. PubMed
Ohayon M.M. Prevalence of hallucinations and their pathological associations in the general population. Psychiatry Res. 2000;97:153–164. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(00)00227-4. PubMed DOI
Lewandowski K.E., DePaola J., Camsari G.B., Cohen B.M., Ongur D. Tactile, olfactory, and gustatory hallucinations in psychotic disorders: A descriptive study. Ann. Acad. Med. Singap. 2009;38:383–387. PubMed
Xue X., Kim M.Y., Wang T., Kuniholm M.H., Strickler H.D. A statistical method for studying correlated rare events and their risk factors. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 2017;26:1416–1428. doi: 10.1177/0962280215581112. PubMed DOI PMC
Lipsitz S.R., Fitzmaurice G.M., Orav E.J., Laird N.M. Performance of Generalized Estimating Equations in practical situations. Biometrics. 1994;50:270–278. doi: 10.2307/2533218. PubMed DOI