Mental simulation of the factual and the illusory in negation processing: evidence from anticipatory eye movements on a blank screen

. 2024 Feb 03 ; 14 (1) : 2844. [epub] 20240203

Jazyk angličtina Země Anglie, Velká Británie Médium electronic

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid38310189

Grantová podpora
FRDF #3725366 University of Auckland
Bilateral project IPCH-2022-04-3316 'Measurement reliability of individual differences in sentence processing: A cross-linguistic perspective (MeRID)', funded by the Croatian science foundation and Swiss national science foundation
UIP-2017-05-6603 'Multilevel approach to spoken discourse in language development' funded by the Croatian Science Foundation

Odkazy

PubMed 38310189
PubMed Central PMC10838274
DOI 10.1038/s41598-024-53353-0
PII: 10.1038/s41598-024-53353-0
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje

How do comprehenders process negative statements such as The fish is not jumping out of the water? Opinions vary. Some argue for two steps, namely that processing starts off with the representation of the positive/illusory [fish jumping out of the water] and then shifts to the (f)actual. To test this idea, we measured fixations on the factual (fish not jumping) versus the illusory (fish jumping) during auditory processing of negation and affirmation. We tested speakers of English (single-cued negation) and Croatian (double-cued negation) and focused on anticipatory fixations in the absence of pictures to indicate the strength of mental simulations. Our findings contribute to negation processing research in two ways. First, dominant anticipatory fixations on the factual suggest a direct rather than a two-step process. Second, time-sensitive insights from two languages call for a finer-grained account of negation processing with negation-specific support of inferences of the factual over the illusory.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Carpenter PA, Just MA. Sentence comprehension: A psycholinguistic processing model of verification. Psychol. Rev. 1975;82(1):45–73. doi: 10.1037/h0076248. DOI

MacDonald MC, Just MA. Changes in activation levels with negation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 1989;15(4):633–642. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.15.4.633. PubMed DOI

Kaup B. Negation and its impact on the accessibility of text information. Mem. Cogn. 2001;29(7):960–967. doi: 10.3758/BF03195758. PubMed DOI

Kaup B, Lüdtke J, Zwaan RA. Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is not open mentally closed? J. Pragmat. 2006;38(7):1033–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2005.09.012. DOI

Kaup B, Yaxley RH, Madden CJ, Zwaan RA, Lüdtke J. Experiential simulations of negated text information. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2007;60(7):976–990. doi: 10.1080/17470210600823512. PubMed DOI

Ćoso B, Bogunović I. Cognitive processing of verbal quantifiers in the context of affirmative and negative sentences: A Croatian study. Res. Lang. 2016;14(3):203–220. doi: 10.1515/rela-2016-0013. DOI

Orenes I, Espino O, Byrne RM. Similarities and differences in understanding negative and affirmative counterfactuals and causal assertions: Evidence from eye-tracking. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2022;75(4):633–651. doi: 10.1177/17470218211044085. PubMed DOI

Barsalou LW. Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 1999;22:577–660. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002149. PubMed DOI

Barsalou LW. Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008;59(1):617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639. PubMed DOI

Zwaan RA, Pecher D. Revisiting mental simulation in language comprehension: Six replication attempts. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e51382. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051382. PubMed DOI PMC

Altmann GT, Kamide Y. The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. J. Mem. Lang. 2007;57(4):502–518. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.12.004. DOI

Kamide Y, Altmann G, Haywood S. The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. J. Mem. Lang. 2003;49:133–156. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00023-8. DOI

Tanenhaus MK, Spivey-Knowlton MJ, Eberhard KM, Sedivy JC. Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science. 1995;268(5217):1632–1634. doi: 10.1126/science.7777863. PubMed DOI

Altmann GT. Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm’. Cognition. 2004;93(2):B79–B87. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.005. PubMed DOI

Dudschig C, Kaup B, Liu M, Schwab J. The processing of negation and polarity: An overview. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2021;50(6):1199–1213. doi: 10.1007/s10936-021-09817-9. PubMed DOI PMC

Zhang H, Vanek N. From ‘No, she does’ to ‘Yes, she does’: Negation processing in negative yes–no questions by Mandarin speakers of English. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2021;42(4):937–967. doi: 10.1017/S0142716421000175. DOI

Zhang H, Wang Y, Vanek N. Negation processing in Chinese-English bilinguals: Insights from the Stroop paradigm and an orientation task. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2022;25(10):3711–3728. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2022.2074274. DOI

Vanek N, Zhang H. On truth and polarity in negation processing: Language-specific effects in non-linguistic contexts. Front. Psychol. 2023;14:1244249. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1244249. PubMed DOI PMC

Déprez V, Tubau S, Cheylus A, Espinal MT. Double negation in a negative concord language: An experimental investigation. Lingua. 2015;163:75–107. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2015.05.012. DOI

Giannakidou A. Negative… Concord? Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory. 2000;18(3):457–523. doi: 10.1023/A:1006477315705. DOI

Maldonado M, Culbertson J. Nobody doesn't like negative concord. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2021;50(6):1401–1416. doi: 10.1007/s10936-021-09816-w. PubMed DOI PMC

Zeijlstra H. Negation in natural language: On the form and meaning of negative elements. Lang. Linguist. Compass. 2007;1(5):498–518. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00027.x. DOI

Giannakidou A. Negative concord and the nature of negative concord items. In: Déprez V, Teresa Espinal M, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Negation. Oxford University Press; 2020. pp. 458–478.

Zovko Dinković I. Negacija u jeziku: Kontrastivna analiza negacije u engleskome i hrvatskome jeziku [Negation in Language: A Contrastive Analysis of Negation in English and Croatian] Hrvatska sveučilišna naklada; 2013.

Blanchette F, Lukyanenko C. Unacceptable grammars? An eye-tracking study of English negative concord. Lang. Cogn. 2019;11(1):1–40. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2019.4. DOI

Dale R, Duran ND. The cognitive dynamics of negated sentence verification. Cogn. Sci. 2011;35(5):983–996. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01164.x. PubMed DOI

Dudschig C, Kaup B. How does ‘not left’ become ‘right’? Electrophysiological evidence for a dynamic conflict-bound negation processing account. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2018;44(5):716–728. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000481. PubMed DOI

Hasson U, Glucksberg S. Does understanding negation entail affirmation?: An examination of negated metaphors. J. Pragmat. 2006;38:1015–1032. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2005.12.005. DOI

Lüdtke J, Friedrich C, De Filippis M, Kaup B. Event-related potential correlates of negation in a sentence-picture verification paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008;20(8):1355–1370. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20093. PubMed DOI

Tian Y, Ferguson H, Breheny H. Processing negation without context—Why and when we represent the positive argument. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;31:683–698. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1140214. DOI

Du P, Liu D, Zhang L, Hitchman G, Lin C. The processing of contradictory and non-contradictory negative sentences. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2014;26(4):461–472. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.903957. DOI

Mayo R, Schul Y, Burnstein E. ‘I am not guilty’ vs ‘I am innocent’: Successful negation may depend on the schema used for its encoding. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2004;40(4):433–449. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.07.008. DOI

Orenes I, Beltrán D, Santamaría C. How negation is understood: Evidence from the visual world paradigm. J. Mem. Lang. 2014;74:36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.04.001. DOI

Tian Y, Breheny R, Ferguson H. Why we simulate negated information: A dynamic pragmatic account. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2010;63(12):2305–2312. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.525712. PubMed DOI

Clark HH, Chase WG. On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cogn. Psychol. 1972;3:472–517. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90019-9. DOI

Kukona A, Fang S-Y, Aicher KA, Chen H, Magnuson JS. The time course of anticipatory constraint integration. Cognition. 2011;119(1):23–42. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.002. PubMed DOI PMC

Kamide Y, Scheepers C, Altmann GT. Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2003;32:37–55. doi: 10.1023/A:1021933015362. PubMed DOI

Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A. GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behav. Res. Meth. Instr. Comp. 1996;28:1–11. doi: 10.3758/BF03203630. DOI

Szekely A, et al. A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. J. Mem. Lang. 2004;51(2):247–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.002. PubMed DOI PMC

Papoutsaki, A., Laskey, J. & Huang, J. Searchgazer: Webcam eye tracking for remote studies of web search. In Proceedings of the 2017 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval, 17–26 (2017).

Stone K, Lago S, Schad D. Divergence point analyses of visual world data: Applications to bilingual research. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2020;24(5):833–841. doi: 10.1017/S1366728920000607. DOI

Barr DJ. Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. J. Mem. Lang. 2008;59(4):457–474. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.002. DOI

Veríssimo J, Clahsen H. Variables and similarity in linguistic generalization: Evidence from inflectional classes in Portuguese. J. Mem. Lang. 2014;76:61–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.06.001. DOI

Van Bergen G, Flecken M. Putting things in new places: Linguistic experience modulates the predictive power of placement verb semantics. J. Mem. Lang. 2017;92:26–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.05.003. DOI

Rommers J, Meyer AS, Praamstra P, Huettig F. The contents of predictions in sentence comprehension: Activation of the shape of objects before they are referred to. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(3):437–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.002. PubMed DOI

Richardson DC, Spivey MJ. Representation, space and Hollywood Squares: Looking at things that aren't there anymore. Cognition. 2000;76(3):269–295. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00084-6. PubMed DOI

Henderson JM, Ferreira F. Scene perception for psycholinguists. In: Henderson JM, Ferreira F, editors. The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye Movements and the Visual World. Psychology Press; 2004. pp. 1–58.

Altmann GT, Kamide Y. Now you see it, now you don't: Mediating the mapping between language and the visual world. In: Henderson JM, Ferreira F, editors. The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye Movements and the Visual World. Psychology Press; 2004. pp. 347–386.

Altmann GT, Kamide Y. Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition. 1999;73(3):247–264. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00059-1. PubMed DOI

Lynott D, Connell L, Brysbaert M, et al. (2020) The Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: Multidimensional measures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English words. Behav. Res. Methods. 2020;52:1271–1291. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01316-z. PubMed DOI PMC

Ćoso B, Bogunović I. The role of linguistic cues in bilingual negation processing. Int. J. Biling. 2019;23(1):21–36. doi: 10.1177/1367006917698840. DOI

Beltrán D, Morera Y, García-Marco E, Vega MD. Brain inhibitory mechanisms are involved in the processing of sentential negation, regardless of its content. Evidence from EEG theta and beta rhythms. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:1782. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01782. PubMed DOI PMC

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...