Imprint of ancestral and modern threats in human mind - experience of fear, disgust, and anger
Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection
Typ dokumentu časopisecké články
PubMed
39881690
PubMed Central
PMC11774860
DOI
10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1520224
Knihovny.cz E-zdroje
- Klíčová slova
- COVID-19, behavioral immune system, evolutionary psychology, fear module, pandemic,
- Publikační typ
- časopisecké články MeSH
INTRODUCTION: Threats to our survival are often posed by the environment in which humans have evolved or live today. Animal and human ancestors developed complex physiological and behavioral response systems to cope with two types of threats: immediate physical harm from predators or conspecifics, triggering fear, and the risk of infections from parasites and pathogens leading to the evolution of the behavioral immune system (BIS) with disgust as the key emotion. Here we ask whether the BIS has adapted to protect us from pandemic risks or poisoning by modern toxic substances. METHODS: We have developed a survey comprised of 60 vignettes describing threats evoking fear and disgust belonging to one of the three main categories of threats: (1) ancestral, (2) modern, and (3) pandemic of airborne disease. Each vignette was evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale based on fear, disgust, and anger. Respondents also completed an assessment battery. RESULTS: The results show that the strongest fear is triggered by modern threats (electricity, car accidents), while the highest disgust is evoked by ancient threats (body waste products, worms). Disgust does not respond to modern threat stimuli such as toxic substances or radioactivity as these evoke mainly fear and anger. A discriminant factor analysis classified nine out of 10 pandemic disgust vignettes into the ancestral disgust category, convincingly assigning the pandemic disgust threats to the ancestral type. Gender, age, and type of education were significant moderators of emotional responses across all threat categories. DISCUSSION: Our study reveals that while fear is more context-dependent, particularly triggered by modern threats, disgust operates on an evolutionarily hardwired basis, making it less effective against contemporary risks. Furthermore, disgust experienced during a pandemic outbreak is more closely aligned with ancestral disgust-related threats tapping into evolutionary ancient survival circuits of the BIS. However, as disgust declines with age, the brain must adaptatively shift the emotional processing from disgust to fear to protect older adults from contamination risks. Finally, our study reveals that pandemic fear is better predicted by specific behaviors rather than general anxiety, suggesting a need for new assessments.
Department of Psychology and Social Sciences Ambis University Prague Czechia
Department of Zoology Faculty of Science Charles University Prague Czechia
Zobrazit více v PubMed
Afroz A., Siddiquea B. N., Chowdhury H. A., Jackson T. N., Watt A. D. (2024). Snakebite envenoming: a systematic review and meta-analysis of global morbidity and mortality. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 18:e0012080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0012080, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Ahorsu D. K., Lin C. Y., Imani V., Saffari M., Griffiths M. D., Pakpour A. H. (2022). The fear of COVID-19 scale: development and initial validation. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 20, 1537–1545. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Ashley V., Swick D. (2019). Angry and fearful face conflict effects in post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Psychol. 10:136. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00136, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Barrett R., Kuzawa C. W., McDade T., Armelagos G. J. (1998). Emerging and re-emerging infectious diseases: the third epidemiologic transition. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 27, 247–271. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.27.1.247 DOI
Beggs C. (2003). The airborne transmission of infection in hospital buildings: fact or fiction? Indoor Built. Environ. 12, 9–18. doi: 10.1177/1420326X03012001002 DOI
Benedictow O. J. (2021). The complete history of the black death. Martlesham: Boydell and Brewer.
Bertels J., Bourguignon M., De Heering A., Chetail F., De Tiège X., Cleeremans A., et al. . (2020). Snakes elicit specific neural responses in the human infant brain. Sci. Rep. 10:7443. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-63619-y, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Blakey S. M., Reuman L., Jacoby R. J., Abramowitz J. S. (2015). Tracing “Fearbola”: psychological predictors of anxious responding to the threat of ebola. Cogn. Ther. Res. 39, 816–825. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9701-9, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Brooke J., Jackson D. (2020). Older people and COVID-19: isolation, risk, and ageism. J. Clin. Nurs. 29, 2044–2046. doi: 10.1111/jocn.15274, PMID: PubMed DOI
Caracci E. (2023). Respiratory particle emission rates from children during speaking. Sci. Rep. 13:18294. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-45615-0, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Caycho-Rodríguez T., Tomás J. M., Barboza-Palomino M., Ventura-León J., Gallegos M., Reyes-Bossio M., et al. . (2022). Assessment of fear of COVID-19 in older adults: validation of the fear of COVID-19 scale. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 20, 1231–1245. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00438-2, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Chang Y., Wang Y., Li W., Wei Z., Tang S., Chen R. (2023). Mechanisms, techniques and devices of airborne virus detection: a review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 20:5471. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20085471, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Christensen R. (2023). Ordinal-Regression models for Ordinal Data. R package version 2023, pp. 12–14. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal.
Cockburn T. A. (1971). Infectious diseases in ancient populations. Curr. Anthropol. 12, 45–62. doi: 10.1086/201168 PubMed DOI
Crosby C. L., Buss D. M., Meston C. M. (2019). Sexual disgust: evolutionary perspectives and relationship to female sexual function. Curr. Sex. Health Rep. 11, 300–306. doi: 10.1007/s11930-019-00219-6 DOI
Curtis V., Aunger R., Rabie T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271, S131–S133. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Dhum M., Herwig U., Opialla S., Siegrist M., Brühl A. B. (2017). Evolutionary and modern image content differentially influence the processing of emotional pictures. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:415. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00415, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Diamond J. (1999). Guns, germs, and steel: the fates of human societies. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.
Díaz A., Beleña Á., Zueco J. (2020). The role of age and gender in perceived vulnerability to infectious diseases. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:485. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17020485, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Dai H., Zhao B. (2022). Association between the infection probability of covid-19 and ventilation rates: an update for sars-cov-2 variants. Build. Simul. 16, 3–12. doi: 10.1007/s12273-022-0952-6 PubMed DOI PMC
Dong J., Xiao T., Xu Q., Liang F., Gu S., Wang F., et al. . (2022). Anxious personality traits: perspectives from basic emotions and neurotransmitters. Brain Sci. 12:1141. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12091141 PubMed DOI PMC
Douple E. B., Mabuchi K., Cullings H. M., Preston D. L., Kodama K., Shimizu Y., et al. . (2011). Long-term radiation-related health effects in a unique human population: lessons learned from the atomic bomb survivors of Hiroshima and Nagasaki. Disaster Med. Public Health Prep. 5, S122–S133. doi: 10.1001/dmp.2011.21, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Drancourt M., Roux V., Dang L., Tran-Hung L., Castex D., Chenal-Francisque V., et al. . (2004). Genotyping, orientalis-likeyersinia pestis, and plague pandemics. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 10, 1585–1592. doi: 10.3201/eid1009.030933, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Eaton W. W., Bienvenu O. J., Miloyan B. (2018). Specific phobias. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 678–686. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30169-X, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Endler N. S., Kocovski N. L. (2001). State and trait anxiety revisited. J. Anxiety Disord. 15, 231–245. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(01)00060-3, PMID: PubMed DOI
Factor R., Yair G., Mahalel D. (2010). Who by accident? The social morphology of car accidents. Risk Anal. 30, 1411–1423. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2010.01423.x, PMID: PubMed DOI
Fessler D. M., Navarrete C. D. (2005). The effect of age on death disgust: challenges to terror management perspectives. Evol. Psychol. 3, 279–296. doi: 10.1177/147470490500300120, PMID: PubMed DOI
Frynta D., Elmi H. S. A., Janovcová M., Rudolfová V., Štolhoferová I., Rexová K., et al. . (2023). Are vipers prototypic fear-evoking snakes? A cross-cultural comparison of Somalis and Czechs. Front. Psychol. 14:1233667. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1233667, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Gamer M., Lemon J., Singh I. F. P. (2019). IRR: various coefficients of interrater reliability and agreement. R package version 0.84.1. 2019. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=irr.
Giner-Sorolla R., Kupfer T., Sabo J. (2018). “What makes moral disgust special? An integrative functional review” in Advances in experimental social psychology. ed. Olson J. M., vol. 57 (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press; ), 223–289.
Haidt J., McCauley C., Rozin P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Pers. Indivd. Differ. 16, 701–713. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7 DOI
Hasegawa A., Tanigawa K., Ohtsuru A., Yabe H., Maeda M., Shigemura J., et al. . (2015). Health effects of radiation and other health problems in the aftermath of nuclear accidents, with an emphasis on Fukushima. Lancet 386, 479–488. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)61106-0, PMID: PubMed DOI
Heretik A., Ritomský A., Novotný V., Pečeňák J. (2009). Restandardizace state-trait anxiety inventory X-2 – úzkostnost jako rys. Cesk. Psychol. 53, 587–599.
Hoehl S., Hellmer K., Johansson M., Gredebäck G. (2017). Itsy bitsy spider…: infants react with increased arousal to spiders and snakes. Front. Psychol. 8:1710. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01710, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Hoeven N., Pappas C., Belser J., Maines T., Zeng H., García-Sastre A., et al. . (2009). Human ha and polymerase subunit pb2 proteins confer transmission of an avian influenza virus through the air. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 106, 3366–3371. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0813172106, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Van Hooff J. C., Devue C., Vieweg P. E., Theeuwes J. (2013). Disgust-and not fear-evoking images hold our attention. Acta Psychol. 143, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.001, PMID: PubMed DOI
Hu M., Yang G., DeBeauchamp J., Crumpton J., Kim H., Li L., et al. . (2020). Ha stabilization promotes replication and transmission of swine h1n1 gamma influenza viruses in ferrets. eLife 9:e56236. doi: 10.7554/eLife.56236, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Hutcherson C. A., Gross J. J. (2011). The moral emotions: a social–functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 719–737. doi: 10.1037/a0022408, PMID: PubMed DOI
Isbell L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. J. Hum. Evol. 51, 1–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012, PMID: PubMed DOI
Jacob A., Shah K., Wu R., Wang H. (2010). Ghrelin as a novel therapy for radiation combined injury. Mol. Med. 16, 137–143. doi: 10.2119/molmed.2009.00154, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Jimenez J. L., Marr L. C., Randall K., Ewing E. T., Tufekci Z., Greenhalgh T., et al. . (2022). What were the historical reasons for the resistance to recognizing airborne transmission during the COVID-19 pandemic? Indoor Air 32:e13070. doi: 10.1111/ina.13070, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Kakehashi M., Yoshinaga F. (1992). Evolution of airborne infectious diseases according to changes in characteristics of the host population. Ecol. Res. 7, 235–243. doi: 10.1007/BF02347092 DOI
Kaňková Š., Takács L., Hlaváčová J., Calda P., Monk C., Havlíček J. (2023). Disgust sensitivity in early pregnancy as a response to high pathogen risk. Front. Psychol. 14:1015927. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1015927, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Karlsen A. R., Antonsen S. (2023). What were we thinking? A scoping review of crisis management pandemic literature (1984–2019). J. Conting. Crisis Manag. 31, 524–544. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.12457 DOI
Kawai N., Koda H. (2016). Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) quickly detect snakes but not spiders: evolutionary origins of fear-relevant animals. J. Comp. Psychol. 130, 299–303. doi: 10.1037/com0000032, PMID: PubMed DOI
LeDoux J. E. (2012). Evolution of human emotion: a view through fear. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 431–442. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00021-0, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
LeDoux J. E. (2014). Coming to terms with fear. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 2871–2878. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400335111, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Lieberman D. (2014). The story of the human body: Evolution, health, and disease. New York: Vintage. PubMed
Lindberg J. C. (2021). ‘J’accuse.!’: The continuous failure to address radiophobia and placing radiation in perspective. J. Radiol. Prot. 41, 459–469. doi: 10.1088/1361-6498/abf9e2, PMID: PubMed DOI
LoBue V., DeLoache J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychol. Sci. 19, 284–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x PubMed DOI
LoBue V., Rakison D., DeLoache J. (2010). Threat perception across the life span. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 375–379. doi: 10.1177/0963721410388801 DOI
Losada-Baltar A., Jiménez-Gonzalo L., Gallego-Alberto L., Pedroso-Chaparro M. D. S., Fernandes-Pires J., Márquez-González M. (2021). "We’re staying at home." association of self-perceptions of aging, personal and family resources, and loneliness with psychological distress during the lockdown period of COVID-19. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. 76, e10–e16. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa048, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Madhav N., Oppenheim B., Gallivan M., Mulembakani P., Rubin E., Wolfe N. (2017). “Pandemics: Risks, impacts, and mitigation” in Disease control priorities: Improving health and reducing poverty. eds. Jamison D. T., Gelbland H., Horton S., Jha P., Laxminarayan R., Mock C. N.. 3rd ed (Washington: World Bank Publications; ), 315–345.
Masataka N., Koda H., Atsumi T., Satoh M., Lipp O. V. (2018). Preferential attentional engagement drives attentional bias to snakes in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) and humans (Homo sapiens). Sci. Rep. 8:17773. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36108-6, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
McLean C. P., Anderson E. R. (2009). Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 496–505. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.003, PMID: PubMed DOI
Meng H., Xu Y., Dai J., Zhang Y., Liu B. (2020). Analyze the psychological impact of COVID-19 among the elderly population in China and its influencing factors. Aging Ment. Health 24, 1973–1980. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112983 DOI
Mobbs D., Hagan C. C., Dalgleish T., Silston B., Prévost C. (2015). The ecology of human fear: survival optimization and the nervous system. Front. Neurosci. 9:121062. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00055, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Musa A., Omyan G., Esmaely F., Shabeeb D. (2019). Radioprotective effect of hesperidin: a systematic review. Medicina 55:370. doi: 10.3390/medicina55070370, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Oaten M., Stevenson R. J., Case T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychol. Bull. 135, 303–321. doi: 10.1037/a0014823, PMID: PubMed DOI
Öhman A., Flykt A., Esteves F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 466–478. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466, PMID: PubMed DOI
Öhman A., Mineka S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108, 483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483, PMID: PubMed DOI
Oksanen J., Blanchet F. G., Friendly M., Kindt R., Legendre P., McGlinn D., et al. . (2020). Vegan: community ecology package. R package version 2.5–7. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=vegan.
Olofsson J. K., Ekström I., Larsson M., Nordin S. (2021). Olfaction and aging: a review of the current state of research and future directions. Iperception 12, 1–24. doi: 10.1177/20416695211020331, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Pakpour A. H., Griffiths M. D., Lin C. Y. (2021). Assessing psychological response to the COVID-19: the fear of COVID-19 scale and the COVID stress scales. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 19, 2407–2410. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00334-9, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Peléšková Š., Polák J., Janovcová M., Chomik A., Sedláčková K., Frynta D., et al. . (2024). Human emotional evaluation of ancestral and modern threats: fear, disgust, and anger. Front. Psychol. 14:1321053. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1321053, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Petrowski K., Paul S., Schmutzer G., Roth M., Brähler E., Albani C. (2010). Domains of disgust sensitivity: revisited factor structure of the questionnaire for the assessment of disgust sensitivity (QADS) in a cross-sectional, representative German survey. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 10:95. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-10-95, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Pinheiro L., Rodrigues J., Borges-Nojosa D. (2016). Formal education, previous interaction and perception influence the attitudes of people toward the conservation of snakes in a large urban center of northeastern Brazil. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 12:25. doi: 10.1186/s13002-016-0096-9, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Polák J., Landová E., Frynta D. (2019). Undisguised disgust: a psychometric evaluation of a disgust propensity measure. Curr. Psychol. 38, 608–617. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-9925-4 DOI
Polák J., Sedláčková K., Nácar D., Landová E., Frynta D. (2016). Fear the serpent: a psychometric study of snake phobia. Psychiatry Res. 242, 163–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.024, PMID: PubMed DOI
Polák J., Sedláčková K., Janovcová M., Peléšková Š., Flegr J., Vobrubová B., et al. . (2022). Measuring fear evoked by the scariest animal: Czech versions of the spider questionnaire and spider phobia beliefs questionnaire. BMC Psychiatry 22:18. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03672-7, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Polák J., Sedláčková K., Landová E., Frynta D. (2020). Faster detection of snake and spider phobia: revisited. Heliyon 6:e03968. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e03968, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Poulton R., Menzies R. G. (2002). Non-associative fear acquisition: a review of the evidence from retrospective and longitudinal research. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 127–149. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00045-6, PMID: PubMed DOI
Prokop P. (2016). “Universal human fears” in Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. eds. Shackelford T. K., Weekes-Shackelford V. A. (New York: Springer International Publishing; ), 1–5.
R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
R Core Team (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Rádlová S., Polák J., Janovcová M., Sedláčková K., Peléšková Š., Landová E., et al. . (2020). Emotional reaction to fear-and disgust-evoking snakes: sensitivity and propensity in snake-fearful respondents. Front. Psychol. 11:31. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00031, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Rakison D. (2009). Does women's greater fear of snakes and spiders originate in infancy? Evol. Hum. Behav. 30, 438–444. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.002, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Rakison D. H. (2018). Do 5-month-old infants possess an evolved detection mechanism for snakes, sharks, and rodents? J. Cogn. Dev. 19, 456–476. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2018.1488717, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Rakison D. H. (2022). “Fear learning in infancy: an evolutionary developmental perspective” in Evolutionary perspectives on infancy. eds. Hart S. L., Bjorklund D. F. (Cham: Springer Nature Switzerland; ), 303–323.
Raoult D., Aboudharam G., Crubézy É., Larrouy G., Lell B., Drancourt M. (2000). Molecular identification by “suicide pcr” of yersinia pestis as the agent of medieval black death. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 12800–12803. doi: 10.1073/pnas.220225197, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Remr J. (2023). Psychometric performance of the fear of COVID-19 scale (FCV-19S) and its validation in Czechia. Sustain. For. 15:8990. doi: 10.3390/su15118990 DOI
Renström E. A., Bäck H. (2021). Emotions during the Covid-19 pandemic: fear, anxiety, and anger as mediators between threats and policy support and political actions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 51, 861–877. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12806, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Rohrmann S., Hopp H., Quirin M. (2008). Gender differences in psychophysiological responses to disgust. J. Psychophysiol. 22, 65–75. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.22.2.65 DOI
Rozin P., Haidt J., McCauley C. R. (1999). “Disgust: the body and soul emotion” in Handbook of cognition and emotion. eds. Dalgleish T., Power M. J. (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons; ), 429–445.
Sayed A., Peng B. (2021). Pandemics and income inequality: a historical review. SN Bus. Econ. 1:54. doi: 10.1007/s43546-021-00059-4 PubMed DOI PMC
Schaller M., Park J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 99–103. doi: 10.1177/0963721411402596 DOI
Seligman M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behav. Ther. 2, 307–320. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(71)80064-3 PubMed DOI
Shapouri S., Martin L. L., Arhami O. (2023). Affective responses to natural and technological disasters; an evolutionary perspective. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 9, 308–322. doi: 10.1007/s40750-023-00224-z, PMID: PubMed DOI
Şimşir Z., Koç H., Seki T., Griffiths M. D. (2022). The relationship between fear of COVID-19 and mental health problems: a meta-analysis. Death Stud. 46, 515–523. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2021.1889097, PMID: PubMed DOI
Skolnick A. J., Dzokoto V. A. (2013). Disgust and contamination: a cross-national comparison of Ghana and the United States. Front. Psychol. 4:91. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00091, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Spielberger C. D., Gorsuch R. L., Lushene R. E., Vagg P. R., Jacobs G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). Washington, DC: Consulting Psychologists Press.
StatSoft, Inc. (2014). STATISTICA (data analysis software system), version 12. Available at: www.statsoft.com.
Stevenson R., Oaten M., Case T., Repacholi B., Wagland P. (2010). Children’s response to adult disgust elicitors: development and acquisition. Dev. Psychol. 46, 165–177. doi: 10.1037/a0016692, PMID: PubMed DOI
Tatem A. J., Rogers D. J., Hay S. I. (2012). Global transport networks and infectious disease spread. Adv. Parasitol. 62, 293–343. doi: 10.1016/S0065-308X(05)62009-X, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Taylor S., Landry C. A., Paluszek M. M., Fergus T. A., McKay D., Asmundson G. J. (2020). Development and initial validation of the COVID stress scales. J. Anxiety Disord. 72:102232. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102232, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Troisi A. (2020). Fear of COVID-19: insights from evolutionary behavioral science. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 17, 72–75. doi: 10.36131/CN20200207 PubMed DOI PMC
Tybur J. M., Lieberman D. (2016). Human pathogen avoidance adaptations. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.06.005 DOI
Tybur J. M., Lieberman D., Griskevicius V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 103–122. doi: 10.1037/a0015474, PMID: PubMed DOI
Tybur J., Inbar Y., Aarøe L., Barclay P., Barlow F., Barra M., et al. . (2016). Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 12408–12413. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607398113, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Venables W. N., Ripley B. D. (2002). Modern applied statistics with S. 4th Edn. New York: Springer.
Vida M. D., Behrmann M. (2017). Subcortical facilitation of behavioral responses to threat. Sci. Rep. 7:13087. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13203-8, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Weisdorf J. L. (2005). From foraging to farming: explaining the Neolithic revolution. J. Econ. Surv. 19, 561–586. doi: 10.1111/j.0950-0804.2005.00259.x DOI
Wheaton M. G., Abramowitz J. S., Berman N. C., Fabricant L. E., Olatunji B. O. (2012). Psychological predictors of anxiety in response to the H1N1 (swine flu) pandemic. Cogn. Ther. Res. 36, 210–218. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9353-3 DOI
Williams G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
World Health Organization (2023). Global status report on road safety 2023. Available at: https://www.who.int/teams/social-determinants-of-health/safety-and-mobility/global-status-report-on-road-safety-2023 (Accessed October 18, 2024).
Yokota K., Mine M., Kondo H., Matsuda N., Shibata Y., Takamura N. (2017). Cancer mortality in residents of the terrain-shielded area exposed to fallout from the Nagasaki atomic bombing. J. Radiat. Res. 59, 1–9. doi: 10.1093/jrr/rrx047, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Yousefzadeh-Chabok S., Ranjbar-Taklimie F., Malekpouri R., Razzaghi A. (2016). A time series model for assessing the trend and forecasting the road traffic accident mortality. Arch. Trauma Res. 5:e36570. doi: 10.5812/atr.36570, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Yang J., Bellgowan P. S., Martin A. (2012). Threat, domain-specificity and the human amygdala. Neuropsychologia 50, 2566–2572. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.001, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Yun H., Yang J., Seo J., Sohn J. (2020). Methodology for sampling and detection of airborne coronavirus including sars-cov-2. Indoor Built. Environ. 31, 1234–1241. doi: 10.1177/1420326X20980160 DOI
Zhang X., Guo Q., Zhang Y., Lou L., Ding D. (2015). Different timing features in brain processing of core and moral disgust pictures: an event-related potentials study. PLoS One 10:e0128531. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128531, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC
Zsido A. N., Arato N., Inhof O., Janszky J., Darnai G. (2018). Short versions of two specific phobia measures: the snake and the spider questionnaires. J. Anxiety Disord. 54, 11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.12.002, PMID: PubMed DOI
Zsido A. N., Hout M. C., Hernandez M., White B., Polák J., Kiss B. L., et al. . (2024). No evidence of attentional prioritization for threatening targets in visual search. Sci. Rep. 14:5651. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-56265-1, PMID: PubMed DOI PMC