Snakes Represent Emotionally Salient Stimuli That May Evoke Both Fear and Disgust

. 2019 ; 10 () : 1085. [epub] 20190509

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid31143154

Humans perceive snakes as threatening stimuli, resulting in fast emotional and behavioral responses. However, snake species differ in their true level of danger and are highly variable in appearance despite the uniform legless form. Different snakes may evoke fear or disgust in humans, or even both emotions simultaneously. We designed three-step-selection experiments to identify prototypical snake species evoking exclusively fear or disgust. First, two independent groups of respondents evaluated 45 images covering most of the natural variability of snakes and rated responses to either perceived fear (n = 175) or disgust (n = 167). Snakes rated as the most fear-evoking were from the family Viperidae (Crotalinae, Viperinae, and Azemiopinae), while the ones rated as the most disgusting were from the group of blind snakes called Typhlopoidea (Xenotyphlopinae, Typhlopinae, and Anomalepidinae). We then identified the specific traits contributing to the perception of fear (large body size, expressive scales with contrasting patterns, and bright coloration) and disgust (thin body, smooth texture, small eyes, and dull coloration). Second, to create stimuli evoking a discrete emotional response, we developed a picture set consisting of 40 snakes with exclusively fear-eliciting and 40 snakes with disgust-eliciting features. Another set of respondents (n = 172) sorted the set, once according to perceived fear and the second time according to perceived disgust. The results showed that the fear-evoking and disgust-evoking snakes fit mainly into their respective groups. Third, we randomly selected 20 species (10 fear-evoking and 10 disgust-evoking) out of the previous set and had them professionally illustrated. A new set of subjects (n = 104) sorted these snakes and confirmed that the illustrated snakes evoked the same discrete emotions as their photographic counterparts. These illustrations are included in the study and may be freely used as a standardized assessment tool when investigating the role of fear and disgust in human emotional response to snakes.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Akaike H. (1998). “Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle,” in Selected Papers of Hirotugu Akaike, eds Parzen E., Tanabe K., Kitagawa G. (New York, NY: Springer; ), 199–213. 10.1007/978-1-4612-1694-0_15 DOI

Angyal A. (1941). Disgust and related aversions. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 36 393–412. 10.1037/h0058254 DOI

Bar M., Neta M. (2006). Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychol. Sci. 17 645–648. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01759.x PubMed DOI

Bar M., Neta M. (2007). Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdala activation. Neuropsychologia 45 2191–2200. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.008 PubMed DOI PMC

Barrett L. F., Lewis M., Haviland-Jones J. M. (eds) (2016). Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Baynes-Rock M. (2017). Human perceptual and phobic biases for snakes: a review of the experimental evidence. Anthrozoös 30 5–18. 10.1080/08927936.2017.1270584 DOI

Biernacki P., Waldorf D. (1981). Snowball sampling: problems and techniques of chain referral sampling. Sociol. Method Res. 10 141–163. 10.1177/004912418101000205 PubMed DOI

Buck J. C., Weinstein S. B., Young H. S. (2018). Ecological and evolutionary consequences of parasite avoidance. Trends Ecol. Evol. 33 619–632. 10.1016/j.tree.2018.05.001 PubMed DOI

Chippaux J. P. (1998). Snake-bites: appraisal of the global situation. Bull. World Health Organ. 76 515–524. PubMed PMC

Cisler J. M., Olatunji B. O., Lohr J. M. (2009a). Disgust, fear, and the anxiety disorders: a critical review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29 34–46. 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.007 PubMed DOI PMC

Cisler J. M., Olatunji B. O., Lohr J. M. (2009b). Disgust sensitivity and emotion regulation potentiate the effect of disgust propensity on spider fear, blood-injection-injury fear, and contamination fear. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychol. 40 219–229. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.10.002 PubMed DOI PMC

Curtis V. (2011). Why disgust matters. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366 3478–3490. 10.1098/rstb.2011.0165 PubMed DOI PMC

Curtis V., Aunger R., Rabie T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271(Suppl. 4), S131–S133. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144 PubMed DOI PMC

Dan-Glauser E. S., Scherer K. R. (2011). The Geneva affective picture database (GAPED): a new 730-picture database focusing on valence and normative significance. Behav. Res. Ther. 43 468–477. 10.3758/s13428-011-0064-1 PubMed DOI

Davey G. C. (1994). Self-reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: the role of disgust sensitivity. Br. J. Psychol. 85 541–554. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02540.x PubMed DOI

de Jong P. J., Merckelbach H. (1998). Blood-injection-injury phobia and fear of spiders: domain specific individual differences in disgust sensitivity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 24 153–158. 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00178-5 PubMed DOI

de Jong P. J., van Overveld M., Peters M. L. (2011). Sympathetic and parasympathetic responses to a core disgust video clip as a function of disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity. Biol. Psychol. 88 174–179. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.07.009 PubMed DOI

de Pinho J. R., Grilo C., Boone R. B., Galvin K. A., Snodgrass J. G. (2014). Influence of aesthetic appreciation of wildlife species on attitudes towards their conservation in Kenyan agropastoralist communities. PLoS One 9:e88842. 10.1371/journal.pone.0088842 PubMed DOI PMC

Doctor R. M., Kahn A. P., Adamec C. (2010). The Encyclopedia of Phobias, Fears, and Anxieties. New York, NY: Infobase Publishing.

Ekman P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 6 169–200. 10.1080/02699939208411068 DOI

Fox E., Griggs L., Mouchlianitis E. (2007). The detection of fear-relevant stimuli: are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion 7 691–696. 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.691 PubMed DOI PMC

Frynta D., Marešová J., Øeháková-Petrù M., Šklíba J., Šumbera R., Krása A. (2011). Cross-cultural agreement in perception of animal beauty: boid snakes viewed by people from five continents. Hum. Ecol. 39 829–834. 10.1007/s10745-011-9447-2 DOI

Frynta D., Šimková O., Lišková S., Landová E. (2013). Mammalian collection on Noah’s ark: the effects of beauty, brain and body size. PLoS One 8:e63110. 10.1371/journal.pone.0063110 PubMed DOI PMC

Gilchrist P. T., Vrinceanu T., Béland S., Bacon S. L., Ditto B. (2016). Disgust stimuli reduce heart rate but do not contribute to vasovagal symptoms. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychol. 51 116–122. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.01.005 PubMed DOI

Goodman L. A. (1961). Snowball sampling. Ann. Math. Stat. 32 148–170.

Grant S., Aitchison T., Henderson E., Christie J., Zare S., Mc Murray J., et al. (1999). A comparison of the reproducibility and the sensitivity to change of visual analogue scales, Borg scales, and Likert scales in normal subjects during submaximal exercise. Chest 116 1208–1217. 10.1378/chest.116.5.1208 PubMed DOI

Grine F. E., Fleagle J. G., Leakey R. E. (eds) (2009). The First Humans: Origin and Early Evolution of the Genus Homo. Dordrecht: Springer, 10.1007/978-1-4020-9980-9 DOI

Guthrie G., Wiener M. (1966). Subliminal perception or perception of partial cue with pictorial stimuli. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 3 619–628. 10.1037/h0023197 PubMed DOI

Haberkamp A., Glombiewski J. A., Schmidt F., Barke A. (2017). The DIsgust-RelaTed-Images (DIRTI) database: validation of a novel standardized set of disgust pictures. Behav. Res. Ther. 89 86–94. 10.1016/j.brat.2016.11.010 PubMed DOI

Haidt J., McCauley C., Rozin P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Pers. Individ. Differ. 16 701–713. 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7 DOI

Isbell L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. J. Hum. Evol. 51 1–35. 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012 PubMed DOI

Isbell L. A., Etting S. F. (2017). Scales drive detection, attention, and memory of snakes in wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus). Primates 58 121–129. 10.1007/s10329-016-0562-y PubMed DOI

Kasturiratne A., Wickremasinghe A. R., de Silva N., Gunawardena N. K., Pathmeswaran A., Premaratna R., et al. (2008). The global burden of snakebite: a literature analysis and modelling based on regional estimates of envenoming and deaths. PLoS Med. 5:e218. 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050218 PubMed DOI PMC

Kimball S., Mattis P. (1995-2005). GIMP 2.8.16E, GNU Image Manipulation Software ∗∗. Available at: https://www.gimp.org/∗ 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050218 PubMed DOI PMC

Klieger D. M., Siejak K. K. (1997). Disgust as the source of false positive effects in the measurement of ophidiophobia. J. Psychol. 131 371–382. 10.1080/00223989709603523 PubMed DOI

Landová E., Bakhshaliyeva N., Janovcová M., Peléšková Š, Suleymanova M., Polák J., et al. (2018a). Association between fear and beauty evaluation of snakes: cross-cultural findings. Front. Psychol. 9:333. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00333 PubMed DOI PMC

Landová E., Poláková P., Rádlová S., Janovcová M., Bobek M., Frynta D. (2018b). Beauty ranking of mammalian species kept in the Prague Zoo: does beauty of animals increase the respondents’ willingness to protect them? Sci. Nat. 105:69. 10.1007/s00114-018-1596-3 PubMed DOI

Landová E., Marešová J., Šimková O., Cikánová V., Frynta D. (2012). Human responses to live snakes and their photographs: evaluation of beauty and fear of the king snakes. J. Environ. Psychol. 32 69–77. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.10.005 DOI

Lang P., Ohman A., Vaitl D. (1988). The International Affective Picture System. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Libkuman T. M., Otani H., Kern R., Viger S. G., Novak N. (2007). Multidimensional normative ratings for the international affective picture system. Behav. Res. Methods 39 326–334. 10.3758/BF03193164 PubMed DOI

Likert R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Arch. Psychol. 22 1–55.

Lišková S., Frynta D. (2013). What determines bird beauty in human eyes? Anthrozoös 26 27–41. 10.2752/175303713X13534238631399 DOI

Lišková S., Landová E., Frynta D. (2015). Human preferences for colorful birds: vivid colors or pattern? Evol. Psychol. 13 339–359. 10.1177/147470491501300203 PubMed DOI

LoBue V., DeLoache J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychol. Sci. 19 284–289. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x PubMed DOI

LoBue V., Deloache J. S. (2011). What’s so special about slithering serpents? Children and adults rapidly detect snakes based on their simple features. Vis. Cogn. 19 129–143. 10.1080/13506285.2010.522216 DOI

Marchewka A., Żurawski Ł., Jednoróg K., Grabowska A. (2014). The nencki affective picture system (NAPS): introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. Behav. Res. Ther. 46 596–610. 10.3758/s13428-013-0379-1 PubMed DOI PMC

Marešová J., Krása A., Frynta D. (2009a). We all appreciate the same animals: cross-cultural comparison of human aesthetic preferences for snake species in Papua New Guinea and Europe. Ethology 115 297–300. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01620.x DOI

Marešová J., Landová E., Frynta D. (2009b). What makes some species of milk snakes more attractive to humans than others? Theor. Biosci. 128 227–235. 10.1007/s12064-009-0075-y PubMed DOI

Masataka N., Hayakawa S., Kawai N. (2010). Human young children as well as adults demonstrate ‘superior’ rapid snake detection when typical striking posture is displayed by the snake. PLoS One 5:e15122. 10.1371/journal.pone.0015122 PubMed DOI PMC

Meno W., Coss R. G., Perry S. (2013). Development of snake-directed antipredator behavior by wild white-faced capuchin monkeys: I. Snake-species discrimination. Am. J. Primatol. 75 281–291. 10.1002/ajp.22106 PubMed DOI

Michałowski J. M., Droździel D., Matuszewski J., Koziejowski W., Jednoróg K., Marchewka A. (2017). The set of fear inducing pictures (SFIP): development and validation in fearful and nonfearful individuals. Behav. Res. Methods 49 1407–1419. 10.3758/s13428-016-0797-y PubMed DOI PMC

Mikels J. A., Fredrickson B. L., Larkin G. R., Lindberg C. M., Maglio S. J., Reuter-Lorenz P. A. (2005). Emotional category data on images from the international affective picture system. Behav. Res. Methods 37 626–630. 10.3758/BF03192732 PubMed DOI PMC

Muller R., Wakelin D. (2002). Worms and Human Disease. New York, NY: CABI Publishing.

Muris P., Mayer B., Huijding J., Konings T. (2008). A dirty animal is a scary animal! Effects of disgust-related information on fear beliefs in children. Behav. Res. Ther. 46 137–144. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.09.005 PubMed DOI

Näsänen R., Ojanpää H., Kojo I. (2001). Effect of stimulus contrast on performance and eye movements in visual search. Vis. Res. 41 1817–1824. 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00056-6 PubMed DOI

Öhman A., Flykt A., Esteves F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130 466–478. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466 PubMed DOI

Öhman A., Mineka S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108 483–522. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483 PubMed DOI

Öhman A., Mineka S. (2003). The malicious serpent: snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 12 5–9. 10.1111/1467-8721.01211 DOI

Öhman A., Soares J. J. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: phobic responses to masked stimuli. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103 231–240. 10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.231 PubMed DOI

Öhman A., Soares S. C., Juth P., Lindström B., Esteves F. (2012). Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: serpents and hostile humans. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24 17–32. 10.1080/20445911.2011.629603 DOI

Oksanen J., Blanchet F. G., Friendly M., Kindt R., Legendre P., McGlinn D., et al. (2017). Vegan: Community Ecology Package. R Package Version 2.4–5.

Olatunji B. O., Huijding J., de Jong P. J., Smits J. A. (2011). The relative contributions of fear and disgust reductions to improvements in spider phobia following exposure-based treatment. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychol. 42 117–121. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.07.007 PubMed DOI

Olatunji B. O., Williams N. L., Tolin D. F., Sawchuck C. N., Abramowitz J. S., Lohr J. M., et al. (2007). The disgust scale: item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychol. Assess. 19 281–297. 10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.281 PubMed DOI

O’Shea M. (2018). The Book of Snakes: A life-Size Guide to Six Hundred Species From Around the World. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Polák J., Landová E., Frynta D. (2018). Undisguised disgust: a psychometric evaluation of a disgust propensity measure. Curr. Psychol. 1–10. 10.1007/s1214 DOI

Polák J., Sedláèková K., Nácar D., Landová E., Frynta D. (2016). Fear the serpent: a psychometric study of snake phobia. Psychiatr. Res. 242 163–168. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.024 PubMed DOI

Pothos E. M., Chater N. (2002). A simplicity principle in unsupervised human categorization. Cogn. Sci. 26 303–343. 10.1016/S0364-0213(02)00064-2 DOI

Pothos E. M., Close J. (2008). One or two dimensions in spontaneous classification: a simplicity approach. Cognition 107 581–602. 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.007 PubMed DOI

Prokop P., Fančovièová J. (2013). Does colour matter? The influence of animal warning colouration in human emotions and willingness to protect them. Anim. Conserv. 16 458–466. 10.1111/acv.12014 DOI

Prokop P., Fančovičová J., Kuèerová A. (2018). Aposematic colouration does not explain fear of snakes in humans. J. Ethol. 36 35–41. 10.1007/s10164-017-0533-9 DOI

Prokop P., Medina-Jerez W., Coleman J., Fančovièová J., Özel M., Fedor P. (2016). Tolerance of frogs among high school students: influences of disgust and culture. EURASIA J. Math. Sci. Tech. 12 1499–1505. 10.12973/eurasia.2016.1241a DOI

Ptáčková J., Landová E., Lišková S., Kubìna A., Frynta D. (2017). Are the aesthetic preferences towards snake species already formed in pre-school aged children? Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 14 16–31. 10.1080/17405629.2016.1144507 DOI

Pyron R. A., Burbrink F. T., Wiens J. J. (2013). A phylogeny and revised classification of Squamata, including 4161 species of lizards and snakes. BMC Evol. Biol. 13:93. 10.1186/1471-2148-13-93 PubMed DOI PMC

R Development Core Team (2010). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Rádlová S., Landová E., Frynta D. (2018). Judging others by your own standards: attractiveness of primate faces as seen by human respondents. Front. Psychol. 9:2439. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02439 PubMed DOI PMC

Rádlová S., Viktorin P., Frynta D. (2016). Barvocuc 2.0, Software for Color Image Analysis. Bethesda: National Institute of Mental Health.

Rasband W. S. (1997–2008). ImageJ. Bethesda: National Institutes of Health.

Riegel M., Żurawski Ł., Wierzba M., Moslehi A., Klocek Ł., Horvat M., et al. (2016). Characterization of the nencki affective picture system by discrete emotional categories (NAPS BE). Behav. Res. Ther. 48 600–612. 10.3758/s13428-015-0620-1 PubMed DOI PMC

Rozin P., Haidt J., McCauley C. R. (1999). “Disgust: the body and soul emotion,” in Handbook of Cognition and Emotion, eds Dalgleish T., Power M. J. (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; ), 429–445. 10.1002/0470013494.ch21 DOI

Sawchuk C. N., Lohr J. M., Tolin D. F., Lee T. C., Kleinknecht R. A. (2000). Disgust sensitivity and contamination fears in spider and blood–injection–injury phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 38 753–762. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00093-5 PubMed DOI

Schaefer H. S., Larson C. L., Davidson R. J., Coan J. A. (2014). Brain, body, and cognition: neural, physiological and self-report correlates of phobic and normative fear. Biol. Psychol. 98 59–69. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.12.011 PubMed DOI PMC

Schindler I., Hosoya G., Menninghaus W., Beermann U., Wagner V., Eid M., et al. (2017). Measuring aesthetic emotions: a review of the literature and a new assessment tool. PLoS One 12:e0178899. 10.1371/journal.pone.0178899 PubMed DOI PMC

Silvia P. J., Barona C. M. (2009). Do people prefer curved objects? Angularity, expertise, and aesthetic preference. Empir. Stud. Arts 27 25–42. 10.2190/EM.27.1.b DOI

Smith M., Davidson J. (2006). It makes my skin crawl.’: the embodiment of disgust in phobias of ‘Nature. Body Soc. 12 43–67. 10.1177/1357034X06061195 DOI

Soares S. C., Esteves F., Flykt A. (2009a). Fear, but not fear-relevance, modulates reaction times in visual search with animal distractors. J. Anxiety Disord. 23 136–144. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.05.002 PubMed DOI

Soares S. C., Esteves F., Lundqvist D., Öhman A. (2009b). Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: evidence from attention and emotion measures. Behav. Res. Ther. 47 1032–1042. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022 PubMed DOI

Soares S. C., Lindström B., Esteves F., Öhman A. (2014). The hidden snake in the grass: superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PLoS One 9:e114724. 10.1371/journal.pone.0114724 PubMed DOI PMC

Sobel I. (1978). Neighborhood coding of binary images for fast contour following and general binary array processing. Comput. Vis. Graph 8 127–135. 10.1016/S0146-664X(78)80020-3 DOI

Souchet J., Aubret F. (2016). Revisiting the fear of snakes in children: the role of aposematic signalling. Sci. Rep. 6:37619. 10.1038/srep37619 PubMed DOI PMC

Stark R., Walter B., Schienle A., Vaitl D. (2005). Psychophysiological correlates of disgust and disgust sensitivity. J. Psychophysiol. 19 50–60. 10.1027/0269-8803.19.1.50 DOI

StatSoft Inc. (2010). Statistica (Data Analysis Software System), Version 9.1. Tulsa, OK: StatSoft Inc.

Tomažič I. (2011). Seventh graders’ direct experience with, and feelings toward, amphibians and some other nonhuman animals. Soc. Anim. 19 225–247. 10.1163/156853011X578901 DOI

Trape J. F., Pison G., Guyavarch E., Mane Y. (2001). High mortality from snakebite in south-eastern Senegal. Trans. R. Soc. Trop. Med. Hyg. 95 420–423. 10.1016/S0035-9203(01)90202-0 PubMed DOI

Uetz P., Hošek J. (2018). The Reptile Database. Available at: http://www.reptile-database.org (accessed November 17, 2015).

Üher J. (1991). On zigzag desings: three levels of meaning. Curr. Anthropol. 32 437–439. 10.1086/203979 DOI

Valenta J. (2008). Jedovatí Hadi. Intoxikace, Terapie. Praha: Galén Press.

Van Le Q., Isbell L. A., Matsumoto J., Nguyen M., Hori E., Maior R. S., et al. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110 19000–19005. 10.1073/pnas.1312648110 PubMed DOI PMC

van Strien J. W., Eijlers R., Franken I. H. A., Huijding J. (2014). Snake pictures draw more early attention than spider pictures in non-phobic women: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biol. Psychol. 96 150–157. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.12.014 PubMed DOI

van Strien J. W., Isbell L. A. (2017). Snake scales, partial exposure, and the snake detection theory: a human event-related potentials study. Sci. Rep. 7:46331. 10.1038/srep46331 PubMed DOI PMC

Woody S. R., McLean C., Klassen T. (2005). Disgust as a motivator of avoidance of spiders. J. Anxiety Disord. 19 461–475. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.04.002 PubMed DOI

Woody S. R., Teachman B. A. (2000). Intersection of disgust and fear: normative and pathological views. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 7 291–311. 10.1093/clipsy.7.3.291 DOI

Wuensch K. L. (2015). What is a Likert Scale? and How Do You Pronounce’ Likert?. Greenville, NC: East Carolina University.

Zsido A. N., Arato N., Inhof O., Janszky J., Darnai G. (2018a). Short versions of two specific phobia measures: the snake and the spider questionnaires. J. Anxiety Disord. 54 11–16. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.12.002 PubMed DOI

Zsido A. N., Deak A., Bernath L. (2018b). Is a snake scarier than a gun? The ontogenetic–phylogenetic dispute from a new perspective: the role of arousal. Emotion [Epub ahead of print]. PubMed

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

Why Are Some Snakes More Terrifying and What Is Behind the Fear?

. 2025 Mar 04 ; 15 (5) : . [epub] 20250304

Hooding cobras can get ahead of other snakes in the ability to evoke human fear

. 2024 Dec 04 ; 112 (1) : 1. [epub] 20241204

No evidence of attentional prioritization for threatening targets in visual search

. 2024 Mar 07 ; 14 (1) : 5651. [epub] 20240307

Attentional, emotional, and behavioral response toward spiders, scorpions, crabs, and snakes provides no evidence for generalized fear between spiders and scorpions

. 2023 Nov 28 ; 13 (1) : 20972. [epub] 20231128

Are vipers prototypic fear-evoking snakes? A cross-cultural comparison of Somalis and Czechs

. 2023 ; 14 () : 1233667. [epub] 20231019

The bigger the threat, the longer the gaze? A cross-cultural study of Somalis and Czechs

. 2023 ; 14 () : 1234593. [epub] 20230927

Animals evoking fear in the Cradle of Humankind: snakes, scorpions, and large carnivores

. 2023 Jul 05 ; 110 (4) : 33. [epub] 20230705

Human emotional evaluation of ancestral and modern threats: fear, disgust, and anger

. 2023 ; 14 () : 1321053. [epub] 20240104

Emotions triggered by live arthropods shed light on spider phobia

. 2021 Nov 15 ; 11 (1) : 22268. [epub] 20211115

The Ultimate List of the Most Frightening and Disgusting Animals: Negative Emotions Elicited by Animals in Central European Respondents

. 2021 Mar 09 ; 11 (3) : . [epub] 20210309

Venomous snakes elicit stronger fear than nonvenomous ones: Psychophysiological response to snake images

. 2020 ; 15 (8) : e0236999. [epub] 20200819

Emotional Reaction to Fear- and Disgust-Evoking Snakes: Sensitivity and Propensity in Snake-Fearful Respondents

. 2020 ; 11 () : 31. [epub] 20200128

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...