Association Between Fear and Beauty Evaluation of Snakes: Cross-Cultural Findings

. 2018 ; 9 () : 333. [epub] 20180316

Status PubMed-not-MEDLINE Jazyk angličtina Země Švýcarsko Médium electronic-ecollection

Typ dokumentu časopisecké články

Perzistentní odkaz   https://www.medvik.cz/link/pmid29615942

According to the fear module theory, humans are evolutionarily predisposed to perceive snakes as prioritized stimuli and exhibit a fast emotional and behavioral response toward them. In Europe, highly dangerous snake species are distributed almost exclusively in the Mediterranean and Caspian areas. While the risk of a snakebite is relatively low in Central Europe, Azerbaijan, on the other hand, has a high occurrence of the deadly venomous Levant viper (Macrovipera lebetina). We hypothesize that co-habitation with this dangerous snake has shaped the way in which humans evaluate snake species resembling it. For that purpose, we asked respondents from the Czech Republic and Azerbaijan to rank photographs depicting 36 snake species according to perceived fear and beauty. The results revealed a high cross-cultural agreement in both evaluations (fear r2 = 0.683, p < 0.0001; beauty: r2 = 0.816, p < 0.0001). Snakes species eliciting higher fear tend to be also perceived as more beautiful, yet people are able to clearly distinguish between these two dimensions. Deadly venomous snakes representing a serious risk are perceived as highly fearful. This is especially true for the vipers and allies (pit vipers) possessing a characteristic body shape with a distinct triangular head and thick body, which was found as the most fear evoking by respondents from both countries. Although the attitude toward snakes is more negative among the respondents from Azerbaijan, their fear evaluation is similar to the Czechs. For instance, despite co-habitation with the Levant viper, it was not rated by the Azerbaijanis as more fearful than other dangerous snakes. In conclusion, agreement in the evaluation of snake fear and beauty is cross-culturally high and relative fear attributed to selected snake species is not directly explainable by the current environmental and cultural differences. This may provide some support for the evolutionary hypothesis of preparedness to fear snakes.

Zobrazit více v PubMed

Abdel-Aal A., Abdel-Baset A. (2010). Venom yield and toxicities of six Egyptian snakes with a description of a procedure for estimating the amount of venom ejected by a single snakebite. Sci. J, King Faisal Univ. 11:1431.

Alekperov A. M. (1977). Zemnovodnye i Presmykayushchiesya Azerbaijana. [Amphibians and Reptiles of Azerbaijan.] Baku: Elm Publications. (in Russian).

Almeida I., Soares S. C., Castelo-Branco M. (2015). The distinct role of the amygdala, superior colliculus and pulvinar in processing of central and peripheral snakes. PLoS ONE 10:e0129949 10.1371/journal.pone.0129949 PubMed DOI PMC

Altman M. N., Khislavsky A. L., Coverdale M. E., Gilger J. W. (2016). Adaptive attention: how preference for animacy impacts change detection. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 303–314. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.01.006 DOI

Alves R. R., Mendonça L. E., Confessor M. V., Vieira W. L., Lopez L. C. (2009). Hunting strategies used in the semi-arid region of northeastern Brazil. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 5:12. 10.1186/1746-4269-5-12 PubMed DOI PMC

Alves R. R. N., Vieira K. S., Santana G. G., Vieira W. L. S., Almeida W. O., Souto W. M. S., et al. . (2012). A review on human attitudes towards reptiles in Brazil. Environ. Monit. Assess. 184, 6877–6901. 10.1007/s10661-011-2465-0 PubMed DOI

Amr Z. S., Disi A. M., Al-Melhim W. N. (1997). Additions to the knowledge of Müller's Snake, Micrelaps muelleri (Boettger, 1880; Squamata: Serpentes: Colubridae). Herpetozoa 10, 163–168.

Arrindell W. A., Eisemann M., Richter J., Oei T. P., Caballo V. E., van der Ende J., et al. . (2003). Phobic anxiety in 11 nations: Part I: Dimensional constancy of the five-factor model. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 461–479. 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00047-5 PubMed DOI

Balakrishnan P. (2010). An education programme and establishment of a citizen scientist network to reduce killing of non-venomous snakes in Malappuram district, Kerala, India. Conserv. Evid. 7, 9–15.

Ballouard J. M., Ajtic R., Balint H., Brito J. C., Crnobrnja-Isailovic J., Desmonts D., et al. (2013). Schoolchildren and one of the most unpopular animals: are they ready to protect snakes? Anthrozoös 26, 93–109. 10.2752/175303713X13534238631560 DOI

Ballouard J. M., Provost G., Barré D., Bonnet X. (2012). Influence of a field trip on the attitude of schoolchildren toward unpopular organisms: an experience with snakes. J. Herpetol. 46, 423–428. 10.1670/11-118 DOI

Bannikov A. G., Darevsky I. S., Ishchenko V. G., Rustamov A. K., Szczerbak N. N. (1977). Guide to Amphibians and Reptiles of the USSR Fauna. Moscow: Prosveshchenie.

Barkow J. H. (1992). Beneath new culture is old psychology: Gossip and social stratification, in The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, eds Barkow J. H., Cosmides L., Tooby J. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 627–637.

Beard K. C. (2002). Basal anthropoids, in The Primate Fossil Record, ed Hartwig W. C. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ), 133–149.

Bjerke T., Østdahl T. (2004). Animal-related attitudes and activities in an urban population. Anthrozoös 17, 109–129. 10.2752/089279304786991783 DOI

Brown J. H. (1973). Toxicology and Pharmacology of Venoms from Poisonous Snakes. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

Calvillo D. P., Hawkins W. C. (2016). Animate objects are detected more frequently than inanimate objects in inattentional blindness tasks independently of threat. J. Gen. Psychol. 143, 101–115. 10.1080/00221309.2016.1163249 PubMed DOI

Chippaux J. P. (1998). Snake-bites: appraisal of the global situation. Bull. World Health Organ. 76:515. PubMed PMC

Chippaux J. P. (2012). Epidemiology of snakebites in Europe: a systematic review of the literature. Toxicon 59, 86–99. 10.1016/j.toxicon.2011.10.008 PubMed DOI

Coborn J. (1991). The Atlas of Snakes of the World. Neptune City, NJ: TFH Publications.

Collins M. A. (1976). Student attitudes towards animals. Am. Biol. Teach. 491–493.

Cook M., Mineka S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 98, 448. 10.1037/0021-843X.98.4.448 PubMed DOI

Cook M., Mineka S., Wolkenstein B., Laitsch K. (1985). Observational conditioning of snake fear in unrelated rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 94:591. 10.1037/0021-843X.94.4.591 PubMed DOI

Crozier W. R. (1999). The meanings of colour: preferences among hues. Pigment Resin Technol. 28, 6–14. 10.1108/03699429910252315 DOI

Davey G. C. (1994). Self-reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: the role of disgust sensitivity. Br. J. Psychol. 85, 541–554. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02540.x PubMed DOI

Dawkins R. (2005). The Ancestor's Tale: A Pilgrimage to the Dawn of Life. New York, NY: Mariner Books.

Deshpande O., Batzog lou S., Feldman M. W., Cavalli-Sforza L. L. (2009). A serial founder effect model for human settlement out of Africa. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 291–300. 10.1098/rspb.2008.0750 PubMed DOI PMC

Egan D. (2007). Snakes of Arabia: a Field Guide to the Snakes of the Arabian Peninsula and Its Shores. Dubai: Motivate Publishing Limited.

El Din S. B. (2006). A Guide to the Reptiles and Amphibians of Egypt. New York, NY: The American University in Cairo Press.

Enquist M., Arak A. (1994). Symmetry, beauty and evolution. Nature 372, 169–172. 10.1038/372169a0 PubMed DOI

Fredrikson M., Annas P., Fischer H., Wik G. (1996). Gender and age differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 33–39. PubMed

Fry B. G., Vidal N., Norman J. A., Vonk F. J., Scheib H., Ramjan S. F. R., et al. . (2006). Early evolution of the venom system in lizards and snakes. Nature 439, 584–588. 10.1038/nature04328 PubMed DOI

Frynta D., Landová E., Lišková S. (2014). Animal beauty, cross-cultural perceptions, in Encyclopaedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research, ed Michalos A. C. (Dordrecht: Springer; ), 179–185.

Frynta D., Lišková S., Bültmann S., Burda H. (2010). Being attractive brings advantages: the case of parrot species in captivity. PLoS ONE 5:e12568. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012568 PubMed DOI PMC

Frynta D., Marešová J., Reháková-Petru M., Šklíba J., Šumbera R., Krása A. (2011). Cross-cultural agreement in perception of animal beauty: boid snakes viewed by people from five continents. Hum. Ecol. 39, 829–834. 10.1007/s10745-011-9447-2 DOI

Glasman L. R., Albarracín D. (2006). Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: a meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. Psychol. Bull. 132:778. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.778 PubMed DOI PMC

Göçmen B., Arikan H., Özbel Y., Mermer A., Çiçek K. (2006). Clinical, physiological and serological observations of a human following a venomous bite by Macrovipera lebetina lebetina (Reptilia: Serpentes). Turkiye Parazitol. Derg. 30, 158–162. PubMed

Hagen T., Laeng B. (2016). The change detection advantage for animals: an effect of ancestral priorities or progeny of experimental design? Iperception 7, 1–17. 10.1177/2041669516651366 PubMed DOI PMC

Hallgren K. A. (2012). Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: an overview and tutorial. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 8:23. 10.20982/tqmp.08.1.p023 PubMed DOI PMC

Hayakawa S., Kawai N., Masataka N. (2011). The influence of color on snake detection in visual search in human children. Sci. Rep. 1:80. 10.1038/srep00080 PubMed DOI PMC

Headland T. N., Greene H. W. (2011). Hunter–gatherers and other primates as prey, predators, and competitors of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, E1470–E1474. 10.1073/pnas.1115116108 PubMed DOI PMC

Hossie T. J., Hassall C., Knee W., Sherratt T. N. (2013). Species with a chemical defence, but not chemical offence, live longer. J. Evol. Biol. 26, 1598–1602. 10.1111/jeb.12143 PubMed DOI

Isbell L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. J. Hum. Evol. 51, 1–35. 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012 PubMed DOI

Janovcová M. (2015). Factors Influencing Worldwide zoo Collections of Lizards, Snakes, Turtles and Crocodiles: Effect of Conservation Status, Body Size and Their Attractiveness to Humans. Prague: Faculty of Science, Charles University in Prague.

Johnson C. R. (1975). Defensive display behaviour in some Australian and Papuan-New Guinean pygopodid lizards, boid, colubrid and elapid snakes. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 56, 265–282. 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1975.tb00269.x DOI

Kasturiratne A., Wickremasinghe A. R., de Silva N., Gunawardena N. K., Pathmeswaran A., Premaratna R., et al. . (2008). The global burden of snakebite: a literature analysis and modelling based on regional estimates of envenoming and deaths. PLoS Med. 5:e218. 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050218 PubMed DOI PMC

Keogh J. S. (1998). Molecular phylogeny of elapid snakes and a consideration of their biogeographic history. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 63, 177–203. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1998.tb01513.x DOI

Khan M. S. (2002). Guide to the Snakes of Pakistan. Frankfurt am Main: Edition Chimaira.

Kühn S., Gallinat J. (2012). The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness. Neuroimage 61, 289–294. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.065 PubMed DOI

Landová E., Marešová J., Šimková O., Cikánová V., Frynta D. (2012). Human responses to live snakes and their photographs: evaluation of beauty and fear of the king snakes. J. Environ. Psychol. 32, 69–77. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.10.005 DOI

Langergraber K. E., Prüfer K., Rowney C., Boesch C., Crockford C., Fawcett K., et al. . (2012). Generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 15716–15721. 10.1073/pnas.1211740109 PubMed DOI PMC

Lipp O. V., Waters A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion 7:192. 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.192 PubMed DOI

Lišková S., Frynta D. (2013). What determines bird beauty in human eyes? Anthrozoös 26, 27–41. 10.2752/175303713X13534238631399 DOI

Lišková S., Landová E., Frynta D. (2015). Human preferences for colorful birds: vivid colors or pattern? Evol. Psychol. 13, 339–359. 10.1177/147470491501300203 PubMed DOI

LoBue V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: the relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion 14:701. 10.1037/a0035898 PubMed DOI

LoBue V., DeLoache J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Dev. Sci. 13, 221–228. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00872.x PubMed DOI

LoBue V., DeLoache J. S. (2011). What's so special about slithering serpents? Children and adults rapidly detect snakes based on their simple features. Vis. Cogn. 19, 129–143. 10.1080/13506285.2010.522216 DOI

LoBue V., Matthews K., Harvey T., Stark S. L. (2014). What accounts for the rapid detection of threat? Evidence for an advantage in perceptual and behavioral responding from eye movements. Emotion 14:816. 10.1037/a0035869 PubMed DOI

LoBue V., Rakison D. H. (2013). What we fear most: a developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Dev. Rev. 33, 285–303. 10.1016/j.dr.2013.07.005 DOI

Makashvili M., Kaishauri N., Azmaiparashvili T. (2014). The role of knowledge in overcoming snake fear. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 152, 184–187. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.09.178 DOI

Malina T., Krecsak L., Warrell D. A. (2008). Neurotoxicity and hypertension following European adder (Vipera berus berus) bites in Hungary: case report and review. QJM 101, 801–806. 10.1093/qjmed/hcn079 PubMed DOI

Mallow D., Ludwig D., Nilson G. (2003). True Vipers: Natural History and Toxinology of Old World Vipers. Malabar, FL: Krieger Publishing Company.

Marais J. (2004). A complete guide to the snakes of southern Africa. Cape Town: Struik.

Marešová J., Frynta D. (2008). Noah's Ark is full of common species attractive to humans: the case of boid snakes in zoos. Ecol. Econ. 64, 554–558. 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2007.03.012 DOI

Marešová J., Krása A., Frynta D. (2009a). We all appreciate the same animals: cross-cultural comparison of human aesthetic preferences for snake species in Papua New Guinea and Europe. Ethology 115, 297–300. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01620.x DOI

Marešová J., Landová E., Frynta D. (2009b). What makes some species of milk snakes more attractive to humans than others?. Theor. Biosci. 128:227. 10.1007/s12064-009-0075-y PubMed DOI

Miller E. R., Gunnell G. F., Martin R. D. (2005). Deep time and the search for anthropoid origins. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 128, 60–95. 10.1002/ajpa.20352 PubMed DOI

Mineka S., Keir R., Price V. (1980). Fear of snakes in wild-and laboratory-reared rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Ani. Learn. Behav. 8, 653–663. 10.3758/BF03197783 DOI

Nalbantsoy A., Erel S. B., Köksal Ç., Göçmen B., Yildiz M. Z., Yavaşoglu N. Ü. K. (2013). Viper venom induced inflammation with Montivipera xanthina (Gray, 1849) and the anti-snake venom activities of Artemisia absinthium L. in rat. Toxicon 65, 34–40. 10.1016/j.toxicon.2012.12.017 PubMed DOI

Nalbantsoy A., Karabay-Yavasoglu N. U., Sayim F., Deliloglu-Gurhan I., Gocmen B., Arikan H., et al. (2012). Determination of in vivo toxicity and in vitro cytotoxicity of venom from the Cypriot blunt-nosed viper Macrovipera lebetina lebetina and antivenom production. J. Venomous Ani. Toxins Include. Trop. Dis. 18, 208–216. 10.1590/S1678-91992012000200011 DOI

New J., Cosmides L., Tooby J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.U.S.A. 104, 16598–16603. 10.1073/pnas.0703913104 PubMed DOI PMC

Öhman A., Flykt A., Esteves F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. 130:466. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466 PubMed DOI

Öhman A., Mineka S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108:483. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483 PubMed DOI

Öhman A., Mineka S. (2003). The malicious serpent snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 12, 5–9. 10.1111/1467-8721.01211 DOI

Öhman A., Soares S. C., Juth P., Lindström B., Esteves F. (2012). Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: Serpents and hostile humans. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 17–32. 10.1080/20445911.2011.629603 DOI

O'Shea M. (1996). Guide to the Snakes of Papua New Guinea. Pt. Moresby: Independent Publishing.

Pandey D. B., Pandey G. S., Evkota K., Goode M. (2016). Public perceptions of snakes and snakebite management: implications for conservation and human health in southern Nepal. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 12, 1–24. 10.1186/s13002-016-0092-0 PubMed DOI PMC

Polák J., Sedláčková K., Nácar D., Landová E., Frynta D. (2016). Fear the serpent: a psychometric study of snake phobia. Psychiatry Res. 242, 163–168. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.024 PubMed DOI

Pook C. E., Joger U., Stümpel N., Wüster W. (2009). When continents collide: phylogeny, historical biogeography and systematics of the medically important viper genus Echis (Squamata: Serpentes: Viperidae). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 53, 792–807. 10.1016/j.ympev.2009.08.002 PubMed DOI

Prokop P., Fančovičová J. (2013). Does colour matter? The influence of animal warning coloration on human emotions and willingness to protect them. Ani. Conserv. 16, 458–466. 10.1111/acv.12014 DOI

Prokop P., Özel M., Uşak M. (2009). Cross-cultural comparison of student attitudes toward snakes. Soc. Anim. 17, 224–240. 10.1163/156853009X445398 DOI

Prokop P., Tunnicliffe S. D. (2008). “Disgusting” animals: primary school children's attitudes and myths of bats and spiders. Eur. J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 4, 87–97. 10.12973/ejmste/75309 DOI

Pyron R. A., Burbrink F. T., Wiens J. J. (2013). A phylogeny and revised classification of Squamata, including 4161 species of lizards and snakes. BMC Evol. Biol. 13:93. 10.1186/1471-2148-13-93 PubMed DOI PMC

Rádlová S., Viktorin P., Frynta D. (2016). Barvocuc 2.0, Software for Color Image Analysis. Available online at: http://www.nudz.cz/en/w_group/wg-on-emotions-and-phobias-triggered-by-animal

R Development Core Team (2010). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Austria.

Samy E. M., Shaaban E. A., Kenawy S. A., Galal M. A., Salama W. H. (2015). Evaluation of the effect of gamma rays on Echis Coloratus snake venom through toxicological, immunological and biological studies. Int. J. Sci. Res. Publ. 5, 79–87.

Schlegel J., Rupf R. (2010). Attitudes towards potential animal flagship species in nature conservation: a survey among students of different educational institutions. J. Nat. Conserv. 18, 278–290. 10.1016/j.jnc.2009.12.002 DOI

Schultz K. D. (1996). A Monograph of the Colubrid Snakes of the Genus Elaphe, Fitzinger. Havlíčkuv Brod: Koeltz Scientific Books.

Schwarz N. (2000). AGENDA 2000 - social judgment and attitudes: Warmer, more social, and less conscious. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 30, 149–176. 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200003/04)30:2<149::AID-EJSP998>3.0.CO;2-N DOI

Soares S. C., Esteves F., Lundqvist D., Öhman A. (2009). Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: evidence from attention and emotion measures. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 1032–1042. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022 PubMed DOI

Sobel I. (1978). Neighborhood coding of binary images for fast contour following and general binary array processing. Comput. Graph. Image Process. 8, 127–135. 10.1016/S0146-664X(78)80020-3 DOI

Spawls S., Branch B., Branch W. R. (1995). The Dangerous Snakes of Africa: Natural History, Species Directory, Venoms, and Snakebite. Sanibel, FL: Ralph Curtis Books.

Spawls S., Howell K., Drewes R., Ashe J. (2002). A Field Guide to the Reptiles of East Africa. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

StatSoft Inc. (2010). Statistica (data Analysis Software System), Version 9.1. Available online at: www.statsoft.com

Steinhoff S. (2017). SnakeDatabase. Available online at: http://snakedatabase.org (accessed 25 March 2017)

Stewart J. R., Stringer C. B. (2012). Human evolution out of Africa: the role of refugia and climate change. Science 335, 1317–1321. 10.1126/science.1215627 PubMed DOI

Stojanov A., Tzankov N., Naumov B., Nöllert A. (2011). Die Amphibien und Reptilien Bulgariens. [Amphibians and reptiles of Bulgaria]. Franfurt am Main: Edition Chimaira. (In German).

Swaroop S., Grab B. (1954). Snakebite mortality in the world. Bull. World Health Organ. 10:35. PubMed PMC

Tierney K. J., Connolly M. K. (2013). A review of the evidence for a biological basis for snake fears in humans. Psychol. Rec. 63:919 10.11133/j.tpr.2013.63.4.012 DOI

Tipples J., Young A. W., Quinlan P., Broks P., Ellis A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 1007–1026. 10.1080/02724980143000659 PubMed DOI

Uetz P., Freed P., Hošek J. (2015). The Reptile Database. Available online at: http://www.reptile-database.org (Accessed March 24, 2015).

Valakos E. D., Pafilis P., Sotiropoulos K., Lymberakis P., Maragou P., Foufopoulos J. (2008). The Amphibians and Reptiles of Greece. Frankfurt am Main: Edition Chimaira.

Valenta J. (2008). Jedovatí hadi. Intoxikace, Terapie. Praha: Galén Press.

Van Le Q., Isbell L. A., Matsumoto J., Nguyen M., Hori E., Maior R. S., et al. . (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 19000–19005. 10.1073/pnas.1312648110 PubMed DOI PMC

Van Le Q., Isbell L. A., Matsumoto J., Quang Le V., Hori E., Tran A. H., et al. . (2014). Monkey pulvinar neurons fire differentially to snake postures. PLoS ONE 9:e114258. 10.1371/journal.pone.0114258 PubMed DOI PMC

Van Strien J. W., Franken I. H., Huijding J. (2014). Testing the snake-detection hypothesis: larger early posterior negativity in humans to pictures of snakes than to pictures of other reptiles, spiders and slugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:691. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00691 PubMed DOI PMC

Vidal N., Hedges S. B. (2002). Higher-level relationships of snakes inferred from four nuclear and mitochondrial genes. C. R. Biol. 325, 977–985. 10.1016/S1631-0691(02)01510-X PubMed DOI

Vuilleumier P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 585–594. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011 PubMed DOI

Wallach V., Williams K. L., Boundy J. (2014). Snakes of the World: A Catalogue of Living and Extinct Species. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Wegener D. T., Carlston D. (2005). Cognitive processes in attitude formation and change, in The Handbook of Attitudes eds Albarracín D., Johnson B. T., Zanna M. P. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; ), 493–542.

Weinstein S. A., Warrell D. A., White J., Keyler D. E. (2011). “Venomous“ Bites from Non-Venomous Snakes: A Critical Analysis of Risk and Management of “Colubrid“ Snakebites. London: Elsevier.

Weiser E., Wollberg Z., Kochva E., Lee S. Y. (1984). Cardiotoxic effects of the venom of the burrowing asp, Atractaspis engaddensis (Atractaspididae, Ophidia). Toxicon 22, 767–774. 10.1016/0041-0101(84)90159-4 PubMed DOI

Weiss L., Brandl P., Frynta D. (2015). Fear reactions to snakes in naïve mouse lemurs and pig-tailed macaques. Primates 56, 279–284. 10.1007/s10329-015-0473-3 PubMed DOI

Whitaker P. B., Shine R. (2000). Sources of mortality of large elapid snakes in an agricultural landscape. J. Herpetol. 121–128. 10.2307/1565247 DOI

Wilcox C. D., Dove S. B., Doss-McDavid W., Greer D. B. (2002). UTHSCSA ImageTool© Ver 3.1. San Antonio: University of Texas Health Science Center.

Wüster W., Peppin L., Pook C. E., Walker D. E. (2008). A nesting of vipers: phylogeny and historical biogeography of the Viperidae (Squamata: Serpentes). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 49, 445–459. 10.1016/j.ympev.2008.08.019 PubMed DOI

Yorek N. (2009). The only good snake is a dead snake: secondary school students' attitudes toward snakes. Biotechnol. Biotechnol. Equip. 23(Suppl. 1), 31–35. 10.1080/13102818.2009.10818358 DOI

Yorzinski J. L., Penkunas M. J., Platt M. L., Coss R. G. (2014). Dangerous animals capture and maintain attention in humans. Evol. Psychol. 12, 534–548. 10.1177/147470491401200304 PubMed DOI

Yousefkhani S. S. H., Yousefi M., Khani A., Pouyani E. R. (2014). Snake fauna of Shirahmad wildlife refuge and Parvand protected area, Khorasan Razavi province, Iran. Herpetol. Notes 7, 75–82.

Nejnovějších 20 citací...

Zobrazit více v
Medvik | PubMed

Why Are Some Snakes More Terrifying and What Is Behind the Fear?

. 2025 Mar 04 ; 15 (5) : . [epub] 20250304

Hooding cobras can get ahead of other snakes in the ability to evoke human fear

. 2024 Dec 04 ; 112 (1) : 1. [epub] 20241204

Attentional, emotional, and behavioral response toward spiders, scorpions, crabs, and snakes provides no evidence for generalized fear between spiders and scorpions

. 2023 Nov 28 ; 13 (1) : 20972. [epub] 20231128

Are vipers prototypic fear-evoking snakes? A cross-cultural comparison of Somalis and Czechs

. 2023 ; 14 () : 1233667. [epub] 20231019

The bigger the threat, the longer the gaze? A cross-cultural study of Somalis and Czechs

. 2023 ; 14 () : 1234593. [epub] 20230927

Animals evoking fear in the Cradle of Humankind: snakes, scorpions, and large carnivores

. 2023 Jul 05 ; 110 (4) : 33. [epub] 20230705

Human emotional evaluation of ancestral and modern threats: fear, disgust, and anger

. 2023 ; 14 () : 1321053. [epub] 20240104

The Ultimate List of the Most Frightening and Disgusting Animals: Negative Emotions Elicited by Animals in Central European Respondents

. 2021 Mar 09 ; 11 (3) : . [epub] 20210309

Venomous snakes elicit stronger fear than nonvenomous ones: Psychophysiological response to snake images

. 2020 ; 15 (8) : e0236999. [epub] 20200819

Emotional Reaction to Fear- and Disgust-Evoking Snakes: Sensitivity and Propensity in Snake-Fearful Respondents

. 2020 ; 11 () : 31. [epub] 20200128

Human evaluation of amphibian species: a comparison of disgust and beauty

. 2019 Jul 01 ; 106 (7-8) : 41. [epub] 20190701

Human Attitude toward Reptiles: A Relationship between Fear, Disgust, and Aesthetic Preferences

. 2019 May 14 ; 9 (5) : . [epub] 20190514

Snakes Represent Emotionally Salient Stimuli That May Evoke Both Fear and Disgust

. 2019 ; 10 () : 1085. [epub] 20190509

Judging Others by Your Own Standards: Attractiveness of Primate Faces as Seen by Human Respondents

. 2018 ; 9 () : 2439. [epub] 20181211

Beauty ranking of mammalian species kept in the Prague Zoo: does beauty of animals increase the respondents' willingness to protect them?

. 2018 Nov 28 ; 105 (11-12) : 69. [epub] 20181128

Najít záznam

Citační ukazatele

Nahrávání dat ...

Možnosti archivace

Nahrávání dat ...